Cogprints

Nested ontology and causal options: A paradigm for consciousness

Merker, Dr. Bjorn (2010) Nested ontology and causal options: A paradigm for consciousness. [Preprint]

Full text available as:

[img]
Preview
PDF - Draft Version
633Kb

Abstract

A brain charged with guiding its body through a complex and lively world from a position of solitary confinement inside its opaque skull faces a set of functional problems whose solution may account for the existence and nature of consciousness. An analysis of the more general and basic of these problems, sensory as well as motor, suggests the utility of implementing a high-level mutual interface between sensory target selection, motor action selection, and motivational ranking of needs at a late stage in the run-up to the brain’s decision about the very next action to take. The three selection processes are subject to a number of mutual dependencies such that a regimen of constraint satisfaction among them would yield gains in behavioral efficiency. The logistics of implementing such a regimen can be simplified by casting the interface in a particular nested, analog format. It would host a running synthetic summary of the rest of the brain’s interpretive labors, reflecting best estimates of the veridical current state of world, body, and needs for purposes of real-time decision making. Detailed scrutiny of the design requirements for such a mechanism discloses that it would be functionally partitioned in a way that defines a conscious mode of operation. Moreover, the design of the mechanism mandates a specific departure from veridicality at a point that makes its functional format match the assumptions of naive realism. Consciousness itself thus introduces a significant, though not insuperable, psychological obstacle to the development of a veridical account of its nature.

Item Type:Preprint
Keywords:Architecture of consciousness, constraint satisfaction, control theory, egocenter, naive realism
Subjects:Biology > Theoretical Biology
ID Code:6853
Deposited By: Merker, Björn
Deposited On:06 Jun 2010 14:35
Last Modified:11 Mar 2011 08:57

References in Article

Select the SEEK icon to attempt to find the referenced article. If it does not appear to be in cogprints you will be forwarded to the paracite service. Poorly formated references will probably not work.

Achlioptas, D., Naor, A., & Peres, Y. (2005). Rigorous location of phase transitions in hard optimization problems. Nature, 435, 759–764.

Adelson, E. H., & Bergen, J. R. (1991). The plenoptic function and the elements of early vision. In M. S. Landy & J. A. Movshon (Eds.), Computational models of visual processing (pp. 1–20). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Altman, J. S., & Kien, J. (1989). New models for motor control. Neural Computation, 1, 173–183.

Andersen, R. A., & Mountcastle, V. B. (1983). The influence of the angle of gaze upon the excitability of the light-sensitive neurons of the posterior parietal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 3, 532–548.

Bernshtein, N.A. (1967). The co-ordination and regulation of movements. Oxford: Pergamon Press.

Blanke, O., Ortigue, S., Landis, T., & Seeck, M. (2002). Stimulating illusory own-body perceptions. Nature, 419, 269–270.

Blanke, O., Landis, T., Spinelli, L., & Seeck, M. (2004). Out-of-body experience and autoscopy of neurological origin. Brain, 127, 243–58.

Brandt, Th., Dichgans, J., & Koenig, E. (1973). Differential effects of central versus peripheral vision on egocentric and exocentric motion perception. Experimental Brain Research, 16, 476–491.

Brooks, R. A. (1986) A robust layered control system for a mobile robot. IEEE Journal on Robotics and Automation, 2, 14–23.

Brouwer, A.-M., & Knill, D. C. (2007). The role of memory in visually guided reaching. Journal of Vision, 7, 1–12. doi:10.1167/7.5.6.

Cabanac, M. (1992). Pleasure: The common currency. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 155, 173–200.

Cabanac, M. (1996). The place of behavior in physiology. In M. J. Fregly & C. Blatteis (Eds.), Handbook of physiology, environmental physiology (pp. 1523–1536). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Cavanagh, P., Hunt, A. R., Afraz, A., & Rolfs, M. (2010). Visual stability based on remapping of attention pointers. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 147–153.

Chang, S. W. C., Papadimitriou, C., & Snyder, L.H. (2009). Using a compound gain field to compute a reach plan. Neuron, 64, 744–755.

Conant, R. C., & Ashby, W. R. (1970). Every good regulator of a system must be a model of that system. International Journal of System Science, 1, 89–97.

Cowan, N. (2001). The Magical Number 4 in Short-term Memory: A Reconsideration of Mental Storage Capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 87–185.

Cox, P. H. (1999). An initial investigation of the auditory egocenter: Evidence for a “cyclopean ear.” Doctoral dissertation, North Carolina State University, Raleigh, NC.

Damasio, A. R. (1989).The brain binds entities and events by multiregional activation from convergence zones. Neural Computation, 1, 123–132.

Dean, P., Porrill, J., & Stone, J. V. (2004). Visual awareness and the cerebellum: possible role of decorrelation control. Progress in Brain Research, 144, 61–75.

Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Le Clec’H, G., Koechlin, E., Mueller, M.,Dehaene-Lambertz, G., van de Moortele, P.-F., & Le Bihan, D. (1998). Imaging unconscious semantic priming Nature, 395, 597–600.

Denton, D. A.,, McKinley, M. J., Farrell, M., & Egan, G. F. (2009). The role of primordial emotions in the evolutionary origin of consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition, 18, 500–514.

Deubel, H., & Schneider, W. X. (1996). Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism. Vision Research, 36, 1827-1837.

Eimer, M., & Schlaghecken, F. (1998). Effects of masked stimuli on motor activation: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1737–1747.

Felleman, D. J., & Van Essen, D. C. (1991). Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 1, 1–47.

Frith, C. (2007). Making up the mind: How the brain creates our mental world. London: Blackwell.

Frith, C. D., Perry, R., & Lumer, E. (1999). The neural correlates of conscious experience : An experimental framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 105–114.

Gaillard, R., Del Cul, A., Naccache, L., Vinckier, F., Cohen, L., & Dehaene, S. (2006). Nonconscious semantic processing of emotional words modulates conscious access. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103:7524 –7529.

Gallistel, C. B. (1999). Coordinate transformations in the genesis of directed action. In B.O.M. Bly & D.E. Rummelhart (Eds.), Cognitive Science (pp. 1–42). New York: Academic Press.

Gorbet, D. J., & Sergio L. E. (2009). The behavioural consequences of dissociating the spatial directions of eye and arm movements. Brain Research, 1284, 77-88.

Green, A. (2003). The science and philosophy of consciousness. Webpage http://www.users.globalnet.co.uk/~lka/conz.htm

Harding, D. E. (1961). On having no head: Zen and the re-discovery of the obvious. London: Arkana

(Penguin).

Harting, J. K., Updyke, B. V., & Van Lieshout, D. P. (1992). Corticotectal projections in the cat: Anterograde transport studies of twentyfive cortical areas. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 328, 379–414.

Helmholtz, H. von (1867). Handbuch der physiologischen Optik, Vol. 3. Leipzig: Leopold Voss.

Henderson, J. M,, Brockmole, J. R., & Gajewski, D. A. (2008). Differential detection of global luminance and contrast changes across saccades and flickers during active scene perception. Vision Research, 48, 16–29

Hering, E. (1879). Spatial sense and movements of the eye. (English translation by C.A. Radde, 1942).

Baltimore: American Academy of Optometry.

Hinton, G. E. (1977). Relaxation and Its Role in Vision. PhD thesis, University of Edinburgh.

Howard, I. P., & Templeton, W. B. (1966). Human spatial orientation. New York: Wiley.

Izard, C. E. (1991). The psychology of emotions. New York: Plenum Press.

Izard, C. E. (2007). Levels of emotion and levels of consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 96–98.

James, W. (1890). Principles of Psychology, London: Macmillan.

Johansson, R. S., Westling, G., Bäckström, A. & Flanagan, J. R. (2001). Eye-hand coordination in object manipulation. Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 6917–6932.

Kawato, M. (1997). Bi-directional theory approach to consciousness. In M. Ito (Ed.), Cognition, computation and consciousness (pp. 233–248). Oxford: Clarendon Press.

Kawato, M. (1999). Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 9, 718–727.

Kawato, M., Hayakawa, H. & Inui, T. (1993). A forward-inverse optics model of reciprocal connections between visual cortical areas. Network: Computation in Neural Systems, 4, 415–422.

Kirby, S. (2002) Learning, bottlenecks and the evolution of recursive syntax. In T. Briscoe (Ed.), Linguistic evolution through language acquisition: Formal and computational models (pp.

173–204). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Körding, K. P., & Wolpert, D. M. (2006) Bayesian decision theory in sensorimotor control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 319–326.

Lau, H. C. & Passingham, R. E. (2007). Unconscious activation of the cognitive control system in

the human prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 5805–5811.

Lehar, S. (2003). The world in your head: A gestalt view of the mechanism of conscious experience. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Lennie, P. (1998). Single units and visual cortical organization. Perception, 27, 889–935.

Liotti, M., Brannan, S., Egan, G., Shade, R., Madden, L., Abplanalp, B, Robillard, R., Lancaster, J., Zamarripa, F.E., Fox, P. T., & Denton, D. (2001). Brain responses associated with consciousness of breathlessness (air hunger). Proceeding of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 98, 2035–2040.

Lopez, L. C. S. (2006). The phenomenal world inside the noumenal head of the giant: Linking the biological evolution of consciousness with the virtual reality metaphor. Revista Eletrônica Informação e Cognição, 5, 204–228.

Luck, S. J., Vogel, E.K. & Shapiro K.L. (1996). Word meanings can be accessed but not reported during the attentional blink. Nature, 383, 616–618.

Mach, E. (1897). Contributions to the analysis of the sensations. La Salle, Illinois: Open Court.

Mack, A. (2003).Inattentional blindness: Looking without seeing. Current Directions in Psychological Science,12, 180-184.

Mandler, G. (1975). Memory storage and retrieval: Some limits on the reach of attention and consciousness. In P. M. A. Rabbitt & S. Dornic (Eds.), Attention and Performance V (pp. 499–516). New York: Academic Press.

Marigold, D. S. (2008). Role of peripheral visual cues in online visual guidance of locomotion. Exercise and Sport Sciences Reviews, 36,145–151.

Marigold, D. S., & Patla, A. E. (2007). Gaze fixation patterns for negotiating complex ground terrain. Neuroscience, 144, 302–313.

Masino, T.. (1992). Brainstem control of orienting movements: Intrinsic coordinate system and underlying circuitry. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 40, 98–111.

McFarland, D. J. (1977). Decision making in animals. Nature, 269, 15–21.

McFarland, D. J., & Sibly, R. M. (1975). The behavioural final common path. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society (London), 270, 265–293.

McGuire, L. M. M., & Sabes, P. N. (2009). Sensory transformations and the use of multiple reference frames for reach planning. Nature Neuroscience, 12, 1056–1061.

Merker, B.. (1997). The common denominator of conscious states: Implications for the biology of consciousness. Archived preprint: http://cogprints.soton.ac.uk (biology; theoretical biology).

Merker, B. (2004). Cortex, countercurrent context, and dimensional integration of lifetime memory. Cortex, 40, 559–576.

Merker, B. (2005). The liabilities of mobility: A selection pressure for the transition to consciousness in animal evolution. Consciousness and Cognition, 14, 89–114.

Merker, B. (2007a). Consciousness without a cerebral cortex: A challenge for neuroscience and medicine. Target article & peer commentary. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 63–110.

Merker, B. (2007b). Grounding consciousness: The mesodiencephalon as thalamocortical base. Author’s response. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 110–134.

Mézard, M., & Mora, T. (2009). Constraint satisfaction problems and neural networks: a statistical physics perspective. Journal of Physiology (Paris), 103, 107–113.

Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 81–97.

Mitchell, H. B. (2007). Multi-sensor data fusion: An introduction. Berlin: Springer-Verlag,

Moll, M., & Miikkulainen, R. (1997). Convergence-zone episodic memory: Analysis and simulations. Neural Networks, 10, 1017–1036.

Morsella, E. (2005). The function of phenomenal states: Supramodular interaction theory. Psychological Review, 112, 1000–1021.

Morsella, E., & Bargh, J. A. (2007). Supracortical consciousness: Insight from temporal dynamics, processing-content, and olfaction. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 100.

Morsella, E., Berger, C. C., & Krieger, S. C. (in press). Cognitive and Neural Components of the Phenomenology of Agency: A Meta-Analytic View. ???

Most, S., Scholl, B., Clifford, E., & Simons, D. (2005). What you see is what you set: Sustained inattentional blindness and the capture of awareness. Psychological Review, 112, 217-242.

Neelon, M. F., Brungart, D. S. & Simpson, B. D. (2004) The isoazimuthal perception of sounds across distance: A preliminary investigation into the location of the audio egocenter. Journal of Neuroscience,

24, 7640–7647.

Pearl, J. (1988). Probabilistic Reasoning in Intelligent Systems: Networks of Plausible Inference (2nd ed.). San Francisco: Morgan Kaufmann.

Philipona, D., O’Regan, J. K., Nadal, J.-P. & Coenen, O. J.-M. D. (2004). Perception of the structure of the physical world using multimodal unknown sensors and effectors. Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems, 16, 945–52.

Rees, G. (2007). Neural correlates of the contents of visual awareness in humans. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society (London) B, 362, 877-886

Rensink, R. A., O’Regan, J. C., & Clark, J. J. (1997). To see or not to see: the need for attention to perceive changes in scenes. Psychological Science, 8, 368–373.

Revonsuo, A. (2006). Inner Presence, Consciousness as a Biological Phenomenon. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

Richards, W., Seung H. S., & Pickard, G. (2006). Neural voting machines. Neural Networks, 19, 1161–1167.

Roelofs, C. O. (1959). Considerations on the visual egocenter. Acta Psychologica, 16, 226–234.

Rojer, A. S., & Schwartz, E. L. (1990). Design considerations for a space-variant visual sensor with complex logarithmic geometry. In Proceedings of the 10th International Conference on

Pattern Recognition (pp. 278–285). Washington, DC: IEEE Computer Society Press.

Rumelhart, D. E., Smolensky, P., McClelland, J. L., & Hinton, G. E. (1986). Schemata and sequential thought processes in PDP models. In D.E. Rumelhart & J.L. McClelland (Eds), Parallel distributed processing: explorations in the microstructure, vol. 2: psychological and biological models (pp. 7–57). Computational models of cognition and perception series. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.

Sachs, E. (1967). Dissociation of learning in rats and its similarities to dissociative states in man. In J. Zubin & H. Hunt (Eds.), Comparative psychopathology: Animal and human (pp. 249–304). New York: Grune & Stratton.

Schacter, D. L. (1989). On the relations between memory and consciousness: Dissociable interactions and conscious experience. In H. L. Roediger, III & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Varieties of Memory and Consciousness: Essays in Honour of Endel Tulving (pp. 356-390). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Schacter, D. L. (1990). Toward a cognitive neuropsychology of awareness: Implicit knowledge and anosognosia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Psychology, 12, 155 – 178.

DOI: 10.1080/01688639008400962

Schopenheuer, A. (1844/1958). The world as will and representation, 2nd ed. Trans. E.F.J. Payne. New York: Dover Publications (paperback, 2 vols).

Shafer, G. (1996). The art of causal conjecture. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

Sibly, R. M., & McFarland, D. J. (1974). A state-space approach to motivation. In J. D. McFarland (Ed.), Motivational control systems analysis (pp. 213-250). London & New York: Academic Press.

Simons, D. J., & Ambinder, M. S. (2005). Change blindness. Theory and consequences. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 44–48.

Singer, W., Zihl, J. & Pöppel, E. (1977). Subcortical control of visual thresholds in humans: Evidence for modality specific and retinotopically organized mechanisms of selective attention. Experimental Brain Research, 29, 173–190.

Smith, M. A. (1997). Sirnulating multiplicative neural processes in non-orthogonal coordinate systems:

a 3-D tensor model of the VOR. Master of Arts thesis, Graduate program in Psychology, York University, North York, Ontario, Canada.

Sparks, D. L. (1999) Conceptual issues related to the role of the superior colliculus in the control of gaze. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 9, 698–707.

Stein, B. E., & Meredith, M. A. C. (1993). The merging of the senses. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

Svarverud, E., Gilson, S. J., & Glennerster, A. (2010). Cue combination for 3D location judgements. Journal of Vision, 10, 1–13. http://journalofvision.org/10/1/5/, doi:10.1167/10.1.5.

Thaler, L., & Goodale, M. A. (2010). Beyond distance and direction: The brain represents target locations non-metrically. Journal of Vision, 10, 1–27. Doi:10.1167/10.3.3.

Trehub, A. (1991). The cognitive brain. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

Trevarthen, C. (1968). Two mechanisms of vision in primates. Psychologische Forschung, 31, 229–337.

Tsang, E. P. K. (1993). Foundations of Constraint Satisfaction. London: Academic Press. Downloadable at: http://cswww.essex.ac.uk/Research/CSP/edward/FCS.html#FCS Download

Turatto, M., Bettella, S., Umiltà, C., & Bridgeman, B. (2003). Perceptual conditions necessary to induce change blindness. Visual Cognition, 10, 233–255.

Velmans, M. (2008). Reflexive monism. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 15, 5–50.

van Gaal, S. , Ridderinkhof, K. R., Fahrenfort, J. J., Scholte,

Metadata

Repository Staff Only: item control page