--- abstract: "It \r\nis proposed that the ability of humans to flourish in diverse \r\nenvironments and evolve complex cultures reflects the following two \r\nunderlying cognitive transitions. The transition from the \r\ncoarse-grained associative memory of Homo habilis to the \r\nfine-grained memory of Homo erectus enabled limited \r\nrepresentational redescription of perceptually similar episodes, \r\nabstraction, and analytic thought, the last of which is modeled as \r\nthe formation of states and of lattices of properties and contexts \r\nfor concepts. The transition to the modern mind of Homo \r\nsapiens is proposed to have resulted from onset of the capacity to \r\nspontaneously and temporarily shift to an associative mode of thought \r\nconducive to interaction amongst seemingly disparate concepts, \r\nmodeled as the forging of conjunctions resulting in states of \r\nentanglement. The fruits of associative thought became ingredients \r\nfor analytic thought, and vice versa. The ratio of \r\nassociative pathways to concepts surpassed a percolation threshold \r\nresulting in the emergence of a self-modifying, integrated internal \r\nmodel of the world, or worldview." altloc: - https://people.ok.ubc.ca/lgabora/papers/gabora_aerts_jmp09.pdf chapter: ~ commentary: ~ commref: ~ confdates: ~ conference: ~ confloc: ~ contact_email: ~ creators_id: - liane.gabora@ubc.ca - diraerts@vub.ac.be creators_name: - family: Gabora given: Liane honourific: 'Dr. ' lineage: '' - family: Aerts given: Diederik honourific: Dr. lineage: '' date: 2009 date_type: published datestamp: 2010-01-30 03:42:33 department: ~ dir: disk0/00/00/67/68 edit_lock_since: ~ edit_lock_until: ~ edit_lock_user: ~ editors_id: [] editors_name: [] eprint_status: archive eprintid: 6768 fileinfo: /style/images/fileicons/application_pdf.png;/6768/1/gabora_aerts_jmp09.pdf full_text_status: public importid: ~ institution: ~ isbn: ~ ispublished: pub issn: ~ item_issues_comment: [] item_issues_count: 0 item_issues_description: [] item_issues_id: [] item_issues_reported_by: [] item_issues_resolved_by: [] item_issues_status: [] item_issues_timestamp: [] item_issues_type: [] keywords: " cognitive \r\ndevelopment, concepts, context, cultural evolution, entanglement, \r\nevolutionary psychology, conceptual integraton, quantum interaction, \r\nSCOP formalism, worldview" lastmod: 2011-03-11 08:57:35 latitude: ~ longitude: ~ metadata_visibility: show note: ~ number: 5 pagerange: ~ pubdom: TRUE publication: Journal of Mathematical Psychology publisher: ~ refereed: TRUE referencetext: "\\item Aerts, D. \r\n(1982). Description of many physical entities Without the paradoxes \r\nencountered in quantum mechanics. \\textit{Foundations of Physics, \r\n12,} 1131-1170. \r\n\\item Aerts, D. (1983). Classical theories and non \r\nclassical theories as a special case of a more general theory, \r\n\\textit{Journal of Mathematical Physics, 24,} 2441-2453.\r\n\r\n\\item Aerts, D. (2007). General quantum modeling of combining concepts: A quantum field model in Fock space. Archive address and link: http://arxiv.org/abs/0705.1740.\r\n\r\n\\item Aerts, D. (2009). Quantum structure in cognition. Journal of Mathematical Psychology (submitted).\r\n\r\nAerts, D., Broekaert, J. and Smets, S. (1999a). The liar paradox in a quantum mechanical perspective. {\\it Foundations of Science, 4}, pp. 115-132.\r\n\r\nAerts, D., Broekaert, J., Smets, S. (1999b). A quantum structure description of the liar paradox. {\\it International Journal of Theoretical Physics, 38}, pp. 3231-3239.\r\n\r\n\r\n\\item Aerts, \r\nD. \\& Gabora, L. (2005a). A theory of concepts and their combinations \r\nI: The structure of the sets of contexts and properties. \r\n\\textit{Kybernetes, 34}(1\\&2), 167-191.\r\n\\item Aerts, D. \\& Gabora, L. \r\n(2005b). A theory of concepts and their combinations II: A Hilbert \r\nspace representation. \\textit{Kybernetes, 34}(1\\&2), 192-221.\r\n\\item \r\nAiello, L. \\& Dunbar, R. (1993). Neocortex size, group size, and the \r\nevolution of language. \\textit{Current Anthropology, 34}, \r\n184-193.\r\n\\item Ashby, F. G., \\& Ell, S. W. (2002). Single versus \r\nmultiple systems of learning and memory. In J. Wixted \\& H. Pashler \r\n(Eds.) \\textit{Stevens? handbook of experimental psychology: Volume 4 \r\nMethodology in experimental psychology}. New York: Wiley.\r\n\\item \r\nBarkow, J., Cosmides, L., \\& Tooby, J. (1992). \\textit{The adapted \r\nmind.} New York: Oxford University Press.\r\n\\item Bar-Yosef, O. (1994). \r\nThe contribution of southwest Asia to the study of the origin of \r\nmodern humans. In M. Nitecki \\& D. Nitecki (Eds.) \\textit{Origins of \r\nanatomically modern humans}, Plenum Press.\r\nBouissac, P. (1998). \r\nConverging parallels: Semiotics and psychology in evolutionary \r\nperspective. \\textit{Theory and Psychology}, 8(6), 731-753.\r\n\r\n\\item \r\nBeltrametti, E. \\& Cassinelli, G. (1981). \\textit{The logic of \r\nquantum mechanics.} Boston: Addison-Wesley.\r\n\\item Bickerton, D. (1990). \\textit{Language and species.} Chicago: University of Chicago \r\nPress.\r\n\r\n\\item \r\nBarsalou, L.W. (1982). Context-independent and context-dependent information in concepts. \\textit{Memory \\& Cognition, 10}, 82-93.\r\n\r\n\\item Bruza, P. \\& Cole, R. (2005). Quantum logic of semantic \r\nspace: An exploratory investigation of context effects in practical \r\nreasoning. In S. Artemov, H. Barringer, A. S. d'Avila Garcez, L. C. \r\nLamb and J. Woods (Eds.) \\textit{We Will Show Them: Essays in Honour \r\nof Dov Gabbay}. College Publications, 1, pp. 339?361.\r\nBruza, P. \r\nKitto, K. Nelson, D. \\& McEvoy, C. (in press). Is there something \r\nquantum-like about the human mental lexicon? Journal of Mathematical \r\nPsychology. \r\n\\item Busemeyer, J. R., Wang, Z., \\& Townsend, J. T. \r\n(2006). Quantum dynamics of human decision making. \\textit{Journal of \r\nMathematical Psychology, 50}, 220-242.\r\n\r\n\\item Busemeyer, J. R. , Matthew, M., \\& Wang, Z. (2006) A Quantum Information Processing Theory Explanation of Disjunction Effects. {\\it Proceedings of the Cognitive Science Society}.\r\n\r\n\\item Busemeyer, J. R. \\& \r\nWang, Z. (2007). Quantum Information Processing Explanation for \r\nInteractions between Inferences and Decisions. \\textit{Proceedings of \r\nthe AAAI Spring Symposium on Quantum Interaction} (pp. 91-97). \r\nStanford University, March 26-28.\r\n\r\n\\item Carey, S. (1985). \r\n\\textit{Conceptual change in childhood}. Cambridge: MIT Press.\r\n\\item \r\nChurchland, P. S. \\& Sejnowski, T. (1992). \\textit{The computational \r\nbrain.} Cambridge MA: MIT Press.\r\n\\item Corballis, M. C. (1991). \r\n\\textit{The lopsided ape: Evolution of the generative mind.} \r\nCambridge: Cambridge University Press.\r\n\\item Davis, J., \\& \r\nRovee-Collier, C. (1983). Alleviated forgetting of a learned \r\ncontingency in 8-week-old infants. \\textit{Developmental Psychology, \r\n19}, 353-365.\r\n\\item Dawkins, R. (1976). \\textit{The selfish gene}. \r\nOxford: Oxford University Press.\r\n\\item Deacon, T. (1997). \\textit{The \r\nsymbolic species}. New York: Norton. [p. 751] \r\n\\item Dennett, D. \r\n(1978). \\textit{Brainstorms}. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.\r\n\\item \r\nDikranjan, D., Giuli, E. \\& Tozzi, A. (1988). Topological categories \r\nand closure operators. \\textit{Questiones Mathematics, 11}(3), \r\n323-337. \r\n\\item Donald, M. (1991) \\textit{Origins of the modern \r\nmind.} Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.\r\n\\item Edelman, G. E. \r\n(1993). \\textit{Bright Air, Brilliant Fire: On the Matter of Mind.} \r\nNew York: Basic Books.\r\n\\item Erdos, P. \\& Renyi, A. (1959). \r\n\\textit{On the random graphs}. 1(6), Institute of Mathematics, \r\nUniversity of Debrecenians, Debrecar, Hungary.\r\n\\item Erdos, P. \\& \r\nRenyi, A. (1960). \\textit{On the evolution of random graphs}. \r\nInstitute of Mathematics, Hungarian Academy of Sciences Publication \r\nnumber 5.\r\n\\item Fauconnier, G. \\& Turner, M. (2002). \\textit{The way \r\nwe think: Conceptual blending and the mind's hidden complexities.} \r\nNew York: Basic Books.\r\n\\item Gabora, L. (1998). A tentative scenario \r\nfor the origin of culture. \\textit{Psycoloquy, 9}(67).\r\n\\item Gabora, \r\nL. (1999). Weaving, bending, patching, mending the fabric of reality: \r\nA cognitive science perspective on worldview inconsistency. \r\n\\textit{Foundations of Science, 3}(2), 395-428. \r\n\\item Gabora, L. \r\n(2000). Conceptual closure: Weaving memories into an interconnected \r\nworldview. In G. Van de Vijver and J. Chandler (Eds.) \r\n\\textit{Closure: Emergent Organizations and their Dynamics. Annals of \r\nthe New York Academy of Sciences 901}, 42-53.\r\n\\item Gabora, L. \r\n(2002a). The beer can theory of creativity. In P. Bentley \\& D. Corne \r\n(Eds.) \\textit{Creative Evolutionary Systems,} (pp. 147-161). San \r\nFrancisco CA: Morgan Kauffman.\r\n\\item Gabora, L. (2002b). Cognitive \r\nmechanisms underlying the creative process. In T. Hewett and T. \r\nKavanagh (Eds.) \\textit{Proceedings of the Fourth International \r\nConference on Creativity and Cognition,} (pp. 126-133). October \r\n13-16, Loughborough University, UK. \r\n\\item Gabora, L. (2003). \r\nContextual focus: A cognitive explanation for the cultural transition \r\nof the Middle/Upper Paleolithic. In (R. Alterman \\& D. Hirsch, Eds.) \r\n\\textit{Proceedings of the 25th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive \r\nScience Society}, Boston MA, 31 July-2 August. Lawrence Erlbaum \r\nAssociates.\r\n\\item Gabora, L. (2004). Ideas are not replicators but \r\ninterrelated networks of them are. \\textit{Biology and Philosophy, \r\n19}(1), 127-143.\r\n\\item Gabora, L. (2005). Creative thought as a \r\nnon-Darwinian evolutionary process. \\textit{Journal of Creative \r\nBehavior, 39}(4), 65-87.\r\n\\item Gabora, L. (2006). Self-other \r\norganization: Why early life did not evolve through natural \r\nselection. \\textit{Journal of Theoretical Biology, 241} (3), \r\n443-450.\r\n\\item Gabora, L. (2008). The cultural evolution of socially \r\nsituated cognition. \\textit{Cognitive Systems Research, 9}(1-2), \r\n104-113.\r\n\\item Gabora, L. \\& Aerts, D. (2002). Contextualizing \r\nconcepts using a mathematical generalization of the quantum \r\nformalism. \\textit{Journal of Experimental and Theoretical Artificial \r\nIntelligence, 14}(4), 327-358.\r\n\\item Gabora, L. \\& Aerts, D. (2005). \r\nEvolution as context-driven actualization of potential: Toward an \r\ninterdisciplinary theory of change of state. \r\n\\textit{Interdisciplinary Science Reviews, 30}(1), 69-88.\r\n\\item \r\nGabora, L., Rosch, E., \\& Aerts, D. (2008). Toward an ecological \r\ntheory of concepts. \\textit{Ecological Psychology, 20}(1), \r\n84-116.\r\n\\item G\\\"ardenfors, P. (2000). \\textit{Conceptual Spaces: The \r\ngeometry of thought.} Cambridge, MA: MIT press. \r\n\\item Gelman, R. \r\n(1993). A rational-constructivist account of early learning about \r\nnumbers and objects. In D. Medin (Ed.) \\textit{Learning and \r\nmotivation,} (pp. 61-96). New York: Academic Press.\r\n\\item Hampton, S. \r\nJ. (2004). Domina mismatches, scruffy engineering, exaptations and \r\nspandrels. \\textit{Theory and Psychology 14}(2), 147-166.\r\n\\item \r\nHancock, P. J. B., Smith, L. S. \\& Phillips, W. A. (1991). A \r\nbiologically supported error-correcting learning rule. {\\it Neural \r\nComputation, 3}(2), 201-212.\r\n\\item Holden, S.B. \\& Niranjan, M. \r\n(1997). Average-case learning curves for radial basis \r\nfunction\r\nnetworks. {\\it Neural Computation, 9}(2), 441-460.\r\n\\item \r\nJohnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). {\\it Mental models.} Cambridge MA: \r\nHarvard University Press.\r\n\\item Kanerva, P. (1988). \\textit{Sparse, \r\ndistributed memory.} Cambridge MA: MIT Press.\r\n\\item Karmiloff-Smith, \r\nA. (1990). Constraints on representational change: Evidence from \r\nchildren?s drawing. \\textit{Cognition, 34}, 57-83.\r\n\\item \r\nKarmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). \\textit{Beyond modularity: A \r\ndevelopmental perspective on cognitive science}. Cambridge MA: MIT \r\nPress.\r\n\\item Kauffman, S. (1993). \\textit{Origins of order}. Oxford: \r\nOxford University Press.\r\n\\item Keil, F. C. (1995). The growth of \r\ncausal understandings of natural kinds. In S. Sperber, D. Premack, \\& \r\nA. Premack (Eds.) \\textit{Causal cognition: A multidisciplinary \r\ndebate,} (pp. 234-262). Cambridge: Oxford University Press.\r\n\\item \r\nKlein, R. (1989). Biological and behavioral perspectives on modern \r\nhuman origins in South Africa. In P. Mellars \\& C. Stringe (Eds.) \r\n\\textit{The human revolution}. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press. \r\n\r\n\\item Langton, C. G. (1992) Life at the edge of chaos. In (C. G. \r\nLangton, C. Taylor, J. D. Farmer \\& S. Rasmussen, eds.) \r\n\\textit{Artificial life II}. Boston: Addison-Wesley.\r\n\\item Leakey, R. \r\n(1984). \\textit{The origins of humankind}. New York: Science Masters \r\nBasic Books.\r\n\\item Leslie, A. (2000). \\textit{Theory of mind as a \r\nmechanism of selective attention}. Rutgers University Tech Report \r\n061.\r\n\\item Lieberman, P. (1991). \\textit{Uniquely human: The \r\nevolution of speech, thought, and selfless behavior.} Boston: Harvard \r\nUniversity Press.\r\n\\item Lu, Y.W., Sundararajan, N. \\& Saratchandran, \r\nP. (1997). A sequential learning scheme for function approximation \r\nusing minimal radial basis function neural networks. {\\it Neural \r\nComputation, 9}(2), 461-478.\r\n\\item Maturana, H. \\& Varela, F. \r\n(1973/1980). \\textit{Autopoeisis and cognition: The realization of \r\nthe living}. Boston: Reidel.\r\n\\item Matzel, L., Collin, C., \\& Alkon, \r\nD. (1992). Biophysical and behavioral correlates of memory storage: \r\nDegradation and reactivation. \\textit{Behavioral Neuroscience, 106}, \r\n954-963.\r\n\\item McBrearty, S. \\& Brooks, A. S. (2000). The revolution \r\nthat wasn?t: A new interpretation of the origin of modern human \r\nbehavior. \\textit{Journal of Human Evolution 39}(5), 453-563.\r\n\\item \r\nMednick, S. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process. \r\n\\textit{Psychological Review, 69,} 220?232.\r\n\\item Mellars, P. (1973). \r\nThe character of the middle-upper transition in south-west France. In \r\nC. Renfrew (Ed.) \\textit{The explanation of culture change}. London: \r\nDuckworth. \r\n\\item Mellars, P. (1989a). Technological changes in the \r\nMiddle-Upper Paleolithic transition: Economic, social, and cognitive \r\nperspectives. In P. Mellars \\& C. Stringer (Eds.) \\textit{The human \r\nrevolution}. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press. \r\n\\item Mellars, \r\nP. (1989b). Major issues in the emergence of modern humans. \r\n\\textit{Current Anthropology, 30}, 349-385. \r\n\\item Mikkulainen \r\n(1997). Neural network perspectives on cognition and adaptive \r\nrobotics. In (A Brown, Ed.) \\textit{Natural language processing with \r\nsubsymbolic neural networks.} Bristol UK, Philadelphia: Institute of \r\nPhysics Press.\r\n\\item Mithen, S. (1996). \\textit{The prehistory of the \r\nmind: A search for the origins of art, science, and religion.} \r\nLondon: Thames \\& Hudson. \r\n\\item Mithen, S. (1998). A creative \r\nexplosion? Theory of mind, language, and the disembodied mind of the \r\nUpper Paleolithic. In S. Mithen (Ed.) \\textit{Creativity in human \r\nevolution and prehistory}. New York: Routledge.\r\n\\item Morowitz, H. J. \r\n(2002). \\textit{The Emergence of Everything: How the World Became \r\nComplex.} Oxford University Press, New York. \r\n\\item Neisser, U. \r\n(1963). The multiplicity of thought. \\textit{British Journal of \r\nPsychology, 54}, 1-14.\r\n\\item Nelson, D. L. \\& McEvoy, C. L. (2007). \r\nActivation at a distance is not spooky anymore: The conjunction of \r\nentangled associative structures and context. \\textit{Proceedings of \r\nthe AAAI Spring Symposium on Quantum Interaction,} Stanford \r\nUniversity, March 26-28.\r\n\\item Nelson, D. L., McEvoy, C. L. \\& \r\nPointer, L. (2003). Spreading activation or spooky action at a \r\ndistance. \\textit{Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, \r\nMemory, and Cognition 29,} 42?51.\r\n\\item Osherson , D. N. \\& Smith, E. \r\nE. (1981). On the adequacy of prototype theory as a theory of \r\nconcepts. \\textit{Cognition, 9}, 35-58.\r\n\\item Piaget 1926). \r\n\\textit{The language and thought of the child}. Hillsdale, NJ: \r\nLawrence Erlbaum.\r\n\\item Piron, C. (1976). \\textit{Foundations of \r\nQuantum Physics.} Reading, MA: W.A. Benjamin.\r\n\\item Rosch (1978). \r\nPrinciples of categorization. In Rosch, E. \\& Lloyd, B. (Eds.) \r\n\\textit{Cognition and categorization}. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence \r\nErlbaum.\r\n\\item Rovee-Collier, C., Hartshorn, K., \\& DiRubbo, M. \r\n(1999). Long-term maintenance of infant memory. \\textit{Developmental \r\nPsychobiology, 35}, 91-102.\r\n\\item Ruff, C., Trinkaus, E. \\& Holliday, \r\nT. (1997). Body mass and encephalization in Pleistocene Homo. \r\n\\textit{Nature, 387,} 173-176.\r\n\\item Sloman, S. (1996). The empirical \r\ncase for two systems of Reasoning. \\textit{Psychological Bulletin, \r\n9}(1), 3-22.\r\n\\item Smolensky, P. (1988). On the proper treatment of \r\nconnectionism. \\textit{Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11,} \r\n1?43.\r\n\\item Soffer, O. (1994). Ancestral lifeways in Eurasia---The \r\nMiddle and Upper Paleolithic records. In M. Nitecki \\& D. Nitecki, \r\n(Eds.) \\textit{Origins of anatomically modern humans.} New York: \r\nPlenum Press. \r\n\\item Spelke, E. S. (2000). Core knowledge. \r\n\\textit{American Psychologist, 55}(11), 1233-1243.\r\n\\item Spoor, F. \r\nLeakey, M. G., Gathogo, P. N., Brown, F. H., Anton, S. C., \r\nMcDougall, I., Kiarie, C., Manthi, F. K. and Leakey, L. N. (2007). \r\nImplications of new early homo fossils from ileret, east of lake \r\nturkana, kenya. \\textit{Nature, 448,} 688-691.\r\n\\item Stringer, C. \\& \r\nGamble, C. (1993). \\textit{In search of the Neanderthals.} London: \r\nThames \\& Hudson. \r\n\\item Thompson, E.(2007). \\textit{Mind in Life - \r\nBiology, Phenomenology and the Sciences of Mind.} Cambridge MA: \r\nHarvard University Press.\r\n\\item Vetsigian, K., Woese, C., \\& \r\nGoldenfeld, N. (2006). Collective evolution and the genetic code. \r\n\\textit{Proceedings of the New York Academy of Science USA, 103,} \r\n10696?10701. \r\n\\item W?chtersh?user, G. (1992). Groundwork for an \r\nevolutionary biochemistry: the iron-sulfur world. \\textit{Prog. \r\nBiophys. Molec. Biol. 58,} 85-201.\r\n\\item Weber, B. H. (1998). \r\nEmergence of life and biological selection from the perspective of \r\ncomplex systems dynamics. In: G. van de Vijver, S. N. Salthe \\& M. \r\nDelpos (Eds.) \\textit{Evolutionary Systems: Biological and \r\nEpistemological Perspectives on Selection and Self-Organization.} \r\nKluwer, Dordrecht. \r\n\\item Weber, B. H. (2000). Closure in the \r\nemergence and evolution of life: Multiple discourses or one? In: J. \r\nL. R. Chandler \\& G. Van de Vijver (Eds.) \\textit{Closure: Emergent \r\nOrganizations and their Dynamics, Annals of the New York Academy of \r\nSciences 901,} pp. 132-138.\r\n\\item Weber, B.H., \\& Depew, J.D. (1996). \r\nNatural selection and self-organization. \\textit{Biology and Philosophy, 11}(1), 33-65.\r\n\\item White, R. (1982). Rethinking the \r\nMiddle/Upper Paleolithic transition. \\textit{Current Anthropology, \r\n23}, 169-189.\r\n\\item White, R. (1993). Technological and social \r\ndimensions of 'Aurignacian-age' body ornaments across Europe. In H. \r\nKnecht, A. Pike-Tay, \\& R. White (Eds.) \\textit{Before Lascaux: The \r\ncomplex record of the Early Upper Paleolithc.} New York: CRC \r\nPress.\r\n\\item Widdows, D. S. (2003). Orthogonal negation in vector \r\nspaces for modeling word meanings and document retrieval. In \r\n\\textit{Proceedings of the 41st Annual Meeting of the Association for \r\nComputational Linguistics} (p. 136-143), Sapporo Japan, July \r\n7-12.\r\n\\item Widdows, D. \\& Peters, S. (2003). Word vectors and \r\nquantum logic: experiments with negation and disjunction. In \r\n\\textit{Eighth Meeting of the Association for the Mathematics of \r\nLanguage} (p. 141-154), Bloomington IN: Association for Computational \r\nLinguistics.\r\n\\item Williams, R. J. P. \\& Frausto da Silva, J. J. R. \r\n(1999). \\textit{Bringing Chemistry to Life: From Matter to Man.} \r\nOxford University Press, Oxford.\r\n\\item Williams, R. J. P. \\& Frausto \r\nda Silva, J. J. R. (2002). The systems approach to evolution. \r\n\\textit{Biochem. Biophys. Res. Comm. 297,} 689-699. \r\n\\item Williams, \r\nR. J. P. \\&Frausto da Silva, J. J. R. (2003). Evolution was \r\nchemically constrained. \\textit{J. Theor. Biol. 220,} 323-343.\r\n\\item \r\nWillshaw, D. J. \\& Dayan, P. (1990). Optimal plasticity from matrix \r\nmemory: What goes up must come down. {\\it Journal of Neural \r\nComputation, 2}, 85-93.\r\n\\item Woese, C.R. (2002). On the evolution of \r\ncells. \\textit{Proceedings of the National Academy of \r\nSciences, \r\n99}(13), 8742-8747. " relation_type: [] relation_uri: [] reportno: ~ rev_number: 24 series: ~ source: ~ status_changed: 2010-01-30 03:42:33 subjects: - cog-psy - cogarch - evol-psy succeeds: ~ suggestions: ~ sword_depositor: ~ sword_slug: ~ thesistype: ~ title: A model of the emergence and evolution of integrated worldviews type: journalp userid: 352 volume: 53