Memon, A., Wark, L., Bull, R. and Koehnken, G. (1997) Isolating the effects of the Cognitive Interview techniques. British Journal of Psychology, 88 (2), 179-198.

Isolating the effects of the Cognitive Interview techniques.

Amina Memon*
Department of Psychology
University of Southampton
Southampton
U.K.

Linsey Wark
University of Southampton

Ray Bull
Department of Psychology
University of Portsmouth
U.K.

Guenter Koehnken
Institut fur Psychologie
University of Kiel
Germany

*Dr. Amina Memon (until 8/1998):

School of Human Development
University of Texas at Dallas,
Richardson, Texas, U.S.A., 75083-0688
email: amemon@utdallas.edu

After 8/1998:

Department of Psychology
University of Southampton
Southampton
S017 1BJ


Keywords

cognitive interview, eyewitness, children questioning

Abstract

The Cognitive Interview (CI) is a procedure designed for use in police interviews involving witnesses. This study tested the most recent version of the CI (Fisher and Geiselman, 1992) which comprises not only `cognitive' techniques such as context reinstatement but also more `social' communication techniques for increasing rapport. Children (8-9 years) viewed a magic show and were interviewed after a short delay of 2 days (Time 1) and/or a longer delay of 12 days (Time 2). At Time 1 the CI produced a significantly greater amount of correct recall than did a structured interview (SI) which was similar to the CI save for the CI's special cognitive techniques. However, at Time 1 the CI also produced significantly more recall errors. At Time 2, no differences occurred between CI and SI recall. There was a significant hypermnesia effect at Time 2 for those groups interviewed twice suggesting an effect of retrieval practice. The Time 1 effects of the CI were found to exist only in the questioning phase of the interview and social and cognitive explanations for the changes in the nature of recall with a CI are considered. Practical implications are discussed in the context of good practice for interviewing child witnesses.

Isolating the effects of the Cognitive Interview techniques.

The ability to obtain full and accurate information is critical in an investigation, yet according to the eyewitness literature, accurate and complete recall is difficult to achieve (e.g. Goodman, Aman, & Hirschman, 1987). The Cognitive Interview (CI) was developed by Fisher and Geiselman in response to the many requests received from police officers for a method of improving recall in witnesses. It draws upon experimental research on memory and is presented as a package of techniques that can be used to facilitate memory search and retrieval (Fisher & Geiselman, 1992). The present study attempts to identify the most effective components of the CI and compare it with current practices for interviewing children (see Bull, 1995a; 1995b). The extent to which variables such as interviewer training, motivation, and effective communication/ rapport with the witness may contribute to gains in information are also explored. The research therefore addresses both theoretical and practical questions.

The CI procedure. The CI procedure essentially comprises four techniques and some strategies for improving communication in an interview (Fisher & Geiselman, 1992; Memon & Koehnken, 1992). One of the principal CI techniques is the mental reinstatement of the physical and personal contexts that existed at the time (see Memon & Bruce, 1985, for a review). Context reinstatement involves (a) emotional elements ("How were you feeling at the time?"), which may work via state-dependent effects (Eich, 1980), (b) perceptual features ("Put yourself back at the scene of the crime and picture the room; how did it smell, what could you hear?") and (c) sequencing elements ("What were you doing at the time?"). The rationale for context reinstatement comes from the encoding specificity principle (e.g., Tulving & Thomson, 1973). The other CI strategies include instructions to search for details extensively (which can lead to the recall of additional relevant information, Geiselman & Fisher, 1988), to recount events in a variety of orders (Loftus & Fathi, 1985) and from a variety of perspectives (e.g., the perspective of the victim, suspect, another witness). These techniques are based on the assumption that memory trace inaccessibility is a result of a limited search.

The four techniques described above formed the basis of the original version of the CI. Further refinements of the CI (Fisher & Geiselman, 1992) include `cognitive techniques' for activating and probing a witness' mental image of the various parts of an event, such as a suspect's face, clothing, objects, etc. A distinction is drawn between conceptual image codes (an image stored as a concept or dictionary definition) and pictorial codes (the mental representation of an image, Paivio, 1971). In addition to the `cognitive' components, the CI in its current form places considerable emphasis on social communication techniques, similar to those recommended to interviewers not employing the CI `cognitive techniques' (Bull, 1992a; Memorandum of Good Practice, 1992). These social communication techniques include `transfer of control' of the interview from the interviewer to the witness. This technique is put into place during the rapport-building phase in several ways. For instance it allows witnesses to dictate the pace of the interview and structure their own recall. This is achieved through use of open questions, by not interrupting witnesses, by timing questions carefully so that they are related to witnesses' retrieval patterns and not to a protocol that an interviewer may be using. For example, if a witness is describing a suspect's face, an appropriate question would be to ask about eye colour rather than to ask about the suspect's shoes. These social communication strategies are said by Fisher and Geiselman (1992) to facilitate the effective implementation of the cognitive CI techniques described earlier. For example, by not interrupting witnesses while they are attempting to recreate context, and by pausing between questions, the interviewer allows the witness time to form an image and to engage in a more exhaustive search, and this may induce more elaborate responses.

In early tests of the CI, the interviewers (students or police officers) received some instruction in use of the `cognitive' components of the CI and collected memory reports of simulated events that students had witnessed two days earlier. In the first study, instructions were given to interviewees in a written form so interviewer-witness interaction was minimal. With relatively brief training interviewers obtained up to 35% more correct information without an increase in errors, as compared to a no-training control group (e.g., Geiselman, Fisher, Mackinnon, & Holland, 1985; 1986). When the revised version of the CI was tested it was found to generate even more information than the original CI (Fisher, Geiselman, Raymond, Jurkevich & Warhaftig, 1987). These results were taken as strong evidence of the effectiveness of the CI. However, upon closer examination of the studies it was apparent that there was one major drawback. Namely, the lack of a suitable control group.

Control group. From a practical perspective, it is important to show that the CI is more effective than the techniques currently recommended for use by police officers and others who conduct investigative interviews. However, an effective control group is needed to demonstrate that the CI cognitive techniques themselves are causing the positive effects and not other factors such as social communication, quality of questioning, rapport building skills or interviewer (or interviewee) motivation. The use of the term `standard' in earlier studies itself implies inferiority. In the present research we sought to construct a control interview (the structured interview, Koehnken, Thurer and Zorberbier, 1994) in which the quality of training in communication and questioning techniques was comparable to the CI and which followed that recommended to professionals who interview children (e.g., 1992 Memorandum of Good Practice). The essence of the Memorandum's guidelines on interview structure is to treat the interview as a procedure in which a variety of interviewing techniques are deployed in relatively discrete phases proceeding from free recall to open and then specific, closed-form questions. Rapport-building, active listening and not interrupting (transfer of control) are also emphasised as important components. The CI techniques fit nicely into this basic framework.

The CI and Children. Given current concerns about the vulnerability of children in criminal proceedings (e.g., Goodman & Bottoms, 1993) and the skills of those who interview child witnesses (Bull, 1992; Clyde, 1992), it is timely to focus on the utility of the CI with this group. In earlier tests, 6-7 year old children did not show any advantage when interviewed with a CI as compared with a structured interview (Memon, Cronin, Eaves and Bull, 1993; Memon, Cronin, Eaves and Bull, in press). A subsequent study suggested that young children had difficulty using some of the CI strategies (e.g. the `reverse order' recall instruction). This could be due to developmental limitations (Flavell and Wellman, 1977) or task demands (Cronin, Eaves, Memon and Bull, 1992; ). More recently, the CI procedure has been further modified for use with child witnesses. Saywitz, Geiselman & Bornstein (1992) evaluated the CI using 7-8-year-olds and 10-11-year-olds as witnesses to a live event. A `practice session' was included (Experiment 2) to familiarise the children with the interview techniques. The interviewers were college students (practice interview) or experienced police officers (interview about main event) who received written instructions and a 2-hour training session which included information about child-appropriate-language, rapport building, interview preparation and procedure, and information on the use of the four original cognitive components of the CI. In the control (standard) condition police officers were instructed to use the techniques they would normally use. The CI led to the recall of more correct details as compared with the standard interview without increasing errors. The CI benefited the older children more than the younger children.

The CI has been investigated in Germany in several studies designed to examine separately any effects of the CI on (i) the incidence of errors and (ii) confabulation (Koehnken, Finger, Nitschke, Hoefer, & Aschermann, 1992; Koehnken, Schimmossek, Aschermann and Hoefer, in press; Koehnken et al, 1994; Mantwill, Aschermann and Koehnken, 1995). With adults, there was an increase in correct details and errors with the CI in two studies (Koehnken et al, in press; Mantwill et al, 1995). With children (aged 9-10 years), it was found the CI did substantially increase the amount of correct information recalled. However, confabulations (the report of details not present in the event) also increased significantly (Koehnken et al., 1992). Several more recent studies where the children have ranged in age from 7-9 years have also reported increases not only in correct information but also in errors with the CI (Milne, Bull, Koehnken, & Memon, 1995; McCauley & Fisher, 1995). From a theoretical and practical perspective it is clearly important to identify why the errors occur and whether or not they can be minimised.

Repeated testing with the CI. So far, research has primarily examined the effects of CI under optimal encoding conditions (a single interview after a relatively brief delay). In the real world a witness may be interviewed many days after the event. Studies of children's delayed recall show that inaccuracies increase over time (Flin, Boon, Knox & Bull, 1992; Poole & White, 1993). Also a witness may be interviewed more than once. While there has been concern addressed about the possible effects of repeated poor interviews with children (Clyde, 1992), laboratory studies of memory and memory development have documented two main positive effects of repeated testing using a variety of stimulus materials. The first is `reminiscence', or the recall of material that did not appear in an earlier test. When this new information exceeds the amount of information that is forgotten, the `hypermnesia' effect has occurred (Payne, 1987). Both these phenomena have been demonstrated in eyewitness contexts using video-taped (Scrivner & Safer, 1988) and staged criminal scenarios (Turtle & Yuille, 1994). We know that memory improves for a short period of time after exposure (Brainerd et al., 1990). Another advantage of repeated testing shortly after an event is that it inoculates against forgetting (Brainerd and Ornstein, 1991; Warren & Lane, 1995).

It is predicted that an initial CI interview will produce hypermnesia effects at second interview on the basis that its techniques such as context reinstatement increase overlap between encoding and retrieval conditions and enrich connections between episodic traces and semantic features (Brainerd, Reyna, Howe, & Kingma, 1990). These factors will improve children's performance on a second test relative to those who had no first test or an inferior first test. An alternative explanation of increased reporting with CI following repeated testing would be that the CI merely shifts a witness' criterion for how much to report. (Note that one of the CI instructions to witnesses is to report everything even that which they are unsure of). Multiple retrieval attempts with the CI may even act as conversational training (Hudson, 1990). Poole and White (1995) made the useful point that a forensic interview is both a memory test and a conversation, and so it is difficult to predict which of these two aspects will most influence recall.

The conclusions of previous CI studies are contradictory. Some have found an increase in correct information, some find an increase in errors, and others get no effects (see Koehnken, Milne, Memon & Bull, 1994, for a review). Moreover, it may be possible for an interviewer armed with a range of `good' interviewing techniques and effective communication skills to achieve the same results as a CI interviewer. The present study aimed to determine, through careful matching of experimental and control group, whether any CI effects reflect improved retrieval searches or whether they are a result of improved interviewer-witness communication. The primary aim of the present study is to test this hypothesis and to assess the nature of inaccuracies that may occur when the CI technique is used with children. In order to directly compare the results of this study with previous research, children aged 8-9 years are used. Previous research has shown this age group show a smaller advantage of testing with CI as compared to older children (Saywitz et al, 1992) and make more errors in their reports (Koehnken et al, 1992; Milne et al, 1995; McCauely and Fisher, 1995).

Method

Participants. One hundred and nine children aged between 8-and 9- years of age participated in the study. There were 53 males and 56 females[1]. There were 29 Cognitive interviews and 32 Structured interviews performed at Time 1. The number of children in each of the four repeat testing groups was as follows: CI/CI = 10; CI/SI = 16; SI/CI = 15 and SI/SI= 13. In addition, the following numbers of children were interviewed at Time 2 only: NCI (None, CI) = 16 and NSI (None, SI) = 17.

The event. Children took part in a magic show, an event chosen because of its interest to children. A local magician was contacted and agreed to take part in the study. The magic show lasted for approximately 9 minutes and there were twelve performances to small groups of children. The magic show was video-recorded.

The magic show was performed to groups of 8-12 children (mean = 9) over a period of two mornings in a room (not their classroom) which the children, although familiar with, did not use often. After a delay of between one or two days (equally distributed across the conditions) some of the children were interviewed in school using a Cognitive or Structured interview. The remainder were not interviewed at that time. After a further delay of between 10 and 13 days (equally distributed between the conditions) all of the children were interviewed (some now for a second time) by either a Cognitive or Structured interviewer. The children interviewed twice had different interviewers at each time.

Design. A 3 (cognitive, structured or no first interview) by 2 (cognitive and structured) design was used to examine the effects of interview technique and repeated testing.

Interviewer training. Four interviewers were trained solely in the Cognitive Interview and four in the Structured Interview.

The Cognitive interviewers were two male and two female students. The Structured interviewers were three female and one male student. The Cognitive interviewers and Structured interviewers were trained separately by a highly qualified researcher (GK) with many years of interview training experience. The structure and content of the training for each group will be described fully as this is something earlier researchers have failed to do. Similarities and differences between the SI and CI should be noted.

Each group of interviewers was trained in two 4-hour sessions that began with an introductory lecture on the importance of the interview in psychological assessment and information gathering in various situations. Both groups were given some guidelines about non-verbal behaviour in the interview (e.g., seating position, eye contact, pauses and speech rate). For each step in the training there was a demonstration role-play excerpt (a CI/SI child witness interview conducted by a trained interviewer ) which was followed by several live practice role-plays (interviewers were asked to choose an event, playing the part of a child and interviewer) and these were video-recorded. Individual feedback was given. There were plenary discussion and question sessions at the end. In addition to active role-plays, the interviewers were encouraged to rehearse mentally the various stages of the interview. For each group the interview was divided into the following phases:

(i) Rapport. Boggs and Eyeberg (1990) pointed out that the essential first phase of the interview is to establish rapport between child and interviewer. She is asked to describe a familiar event, for example, a favourite game. Follow-up comments such as: "that sounds fun, tell me how you play it?" increase rapport and prime the child to give elaborate responses. Both the CI and SI group practised rapport building in this way (cf., Saywitz et al., 1992). An important part of the rapport building was the transfer of control from interviewer to interviewee (which included active listening, not interrupting and effective use of pauses). As part of this transfer of control the interviewer makes it clear that he or she does not have the information about the event but rather it is the child who holds the information. This procedure typically forms part of the CI but it has also been advocated in other interviewing guidelines (e.g. the Memorandum of Good Practice, 1992) and therefore this component formed part of the training for the SI as well as the CI group.

(ii) Free recall phase. The SI interviewers were asked to request a free narrative account from the witness and this was used as a strategy for obtaining information in the subsequent questioning phase. The CI group received identical instructions for the free recall phase, but in addition they were given training in encouraging witnesses to reinstate the context mentally (as described in the introduction) before they began. The CI interviewers also employed the `report everything' instruction at this stage.

(iii) Prompt Phase. At the end of the free recall phase, CI and SI interviewers paused briefly and used one prompt: "Please tell me more" before commencing the questioning phase.

(iv) Questioning Phase. In the next phase both the CI and SI interviewers were asked to use the information reported by the witness in their free recall phase as a guide for follow-up questions. Both the CI and SI interviewers were instructed in the use of appropriate types of questions. They were asked to begin with open questions and then follow these with closed questions. In general interviewers were asked to use the free report to find out who was present at the event and what they did. Where a person was mentioned, interviewers were asked to elicit details about clothing. They were specifically instructed to avoid leading, misleading, and forced-choice questions. The CI interviewers received additional training in the activation and probing of images relating to various parts of the event. (For example, the children were told to "picture the magician's face, and then describe it.").

(v) Second retrieval phase. The purpose of this phase was to examine the effects of additional instructions on the recall of new information. The second retrieval phase of the interview was different for the two groups. The CI interviewers employed the CI `reverse order' recall instruction at this point. This took the following form: "Tell me about the very last thing you remember in the magic show and then what happened before that, and before that, so you're working your way back to the first thing you remember." It was placed towards the end of the interview so that any extra information it elicited could be identified. The SI group also attempted to elicit additional information at this stage by asking children to go through the event again, recalling additional details if possible.

Both groups received a summary of the theoretical background material relevant to their training and a detailed handout containing all the training material to study. Each group was led to believe they were the `experimental group.'

Coding and scoring of the interview transcripts. Using the videos of the 12 magic shows, two research assistants identified the details that could be recalled. This produced some 650 details. The information contained in the verbatim transcript of each interview was checked with the corresponding video and classified as correct, as an error that is, wrongly describing something that was present or did happen) or as a confabulation (e.g. saying a `pig' was present when it was not).

Information was classified into four detail types describing persons, actions, objects or surroundings. Although any classification of detail type is dependent on the sort of event used, there were several concerns that we wanted to address. Firstly we wanted to test the hypothesis that the CI, by encouraging reinstatement of context, merely increases the reporting of details about surroundings (Memon et al., in press). The present coding classification therefore includes a separate count of such details. Secondly, we wanted to examine whether the CI might increase the reporting of all types of erroneous information, or whether errors (if they occurred) may reflect the difficulty children have in giving descriptions about persons as compared to actions (Davies, Tarrant, & Flin, 1989).

The stage at which information was reported in the interview was noted with a distinction being made between free recall (FR), prompt (PR), questioning (QU), and second retrieval (SR) phases. After the information contained in the child's free report was scored for accuracy, recall appearing in the other subsequent sections of the interview (QU, PR and SR phases) was only scored if it was new. This is illustrated in Appendix I.

Two research assistants coded and scored the interview transcripts and any uncertainties were resolved by discussion. In addition to this, ten transcripts were coded by both of the research assistants and inter-coder reliabilities were calculated: inter-coder agreement for total accurate was 96% ( r=.93 p<.0001) and for total errors 89% (r=.86 p<.001).

Results

Two types of measures are reported throughout. Firstly, the absolute number of errors and confabulations are reported. Secondly, instead of simply reporting the absolute number of correct details, these are expressed as a proportion of that available to be recalled because each of the 12 magic shows contained slightly different information (ie., descriptions of the children who helped, what they were wearing, etc.). This will be referred to as `percentage correct' in the results described below.

As is now the convention in research on the CI, the number of correct, incorrect, and confabulated details are also each expressed as a proportion of the total number recalled. The proportion is calculated by dividing the number of each type of detail (i.e. correct, or incorrect and confabulations) by the total number of details reported (i.e., correct plus incorrect plus confabulations). This measure is usually referred to as accuracy (and sometimes as reliability).

First interviews

Amount recalled at Time 1. A series of one-way ANOVAs was performed to test the prediction that the CI would increase the amount of correct and of incorrect information (errors and confabulations)[2]. For percentage correct recall there was a significant effect (F(1, 59) = 6.17, p < .05) with the CI producing more correct recall. Similarly, the absolute numbers of errors was significantly higher in the CI condition (F (1, 59) = 4.17, p < .05). There was no significant effect of type of interview on confabulated details (F < 1). The increase in percentage correct information with the CI was a difference of 20 units of information. This was accompanied by an increase of 3.31 units of erroneous details. So with a control group matched in every respect to the CI save the `cognitive' components of CI, there is a positive effect on the percentage of correct recall. There is, also an increase in the absolute number of errors (although not in the proportion of recall that was inaccurate) and this is considered at length in the discussion.

Accuracy at Time 1. It was found that, on average, 88% of the children's recall was correct with 8% errors and 4% confabulated details. This is consistent with earlier research (Saywitz et al., 1992). A one-way ANOVA (interview type) was performed on the accuracy of recall (amount correct expressed as a function of total amount reported). From this no significant differences in accuracy emerged across interview type (F < 1).

Recall at each phase of the interview at Time 1. The `percentage correct', total number of correct details and absolute amount of inaccurate information reported at each phase of the interview can be seen in Table 1. Most of the accurate information the children recalled was in the FR and QU phases of the interview, as would be expected given information was only scored later if it was new.

Table 1: Percentage correct recall, total correct and errors by interview phase (Time 1).


Cognitive

Percentage correct

Interview

Total

correct

Errors
Structured

Percentage correct

Interview

Total

correct

Errors
Free Recall

M

SD

7.5

4.4

48.1

28.2

2.1

2.3

7.3

3.8

46.4

24.0

2.2

2.0

Prompt

M

SD

0.7

1.8

4.5

11.38

0.1

0.6

0.2

0.6

1.2

3.5

0.0

0.2

Questioning

M

SD

6.6**

2.8

42.7

18.0

6.8**

4.7

4.0

2.3

25.7

14.8

4.0

3.2

Second Retrieval

M

SD

1.0

1.3

6.5

8.4

0.9

1.5

1.3

1.3

8.1

8.5

0.9

1.3

TOTAL

M

SD

15.8*

4.9

101.9

31.9

10.0*

6.6

12.7

4.9

81.5

31.4

7.1

4.6

* p <. 05

** p < .01

Note. The 'percentage correct' measure shows the % of possible accurate recall for event

Previous research with child witnesses has not established where in the interview the effects of CI appear. In the present study the CI instructions were given prior to both the FR and QU phases. As indicated earlier, information from the FR phase was used to probe for further detail in the QU phase. On this basis it would be expected that the CI would show an increase in new information at each of these phases. A series of one-way ANOVAs were performed to look at the `percentage correct' information, errors and confabulations at each phase of the interview[3].

The only phase to yield significant effects of interview type was the question phase. The percentage correct information was significantly higher in the CI as compared to the SI group (F(1, 59) =16.22, p < .001). This was accompanied by a significant increase in errors (F (1, 59) = 8.43, p < .01). There was no effect of interview type on confabulations in the QU phase (F (1, 59 = 1.32, p >.05; means = 3.62 for the CI and 2.59 for the SI).

One question that was of particular interest in this study was the effects of multiple retrieval attempts within a single interview session on recall. We were interested in whether the reverse order recall (CI) instruction could generate any more information than a simple request to try again. There was no beneficial effect of using the CI reverse order recall instruction over a simple instruction to go through the event one more time. In other words there were no significant CI-SI differences in the SR phase (F < 1). Averaging across these two conditions, there were approximately 6 additional units of information generated within the additional retrieval attempt.

Recall by Type of Detail at Time 1 So far the analyses show that the significant CI-SI difference in percentage correct and errors only occurred in the questioning phase. It was important to examine the question phase in more depth to ascertain the types of details (person, action, object and surrounding) being recalled. The next series of analyses breaks down the `percentage correct' recall and errors at the QU phase into types of information recalled (see Table 2) and a series of one-way ANOVAs was conducted to compare performance across CI and SI for each dependent variable (confabulations are not reported in this section as there were no significant main effects).

Table 2: Percentage correct information, total correct and errors in the QU phase by detail type (Time 1)


Cognitive

percentage correct

Interview

Total

correct

Errors
Structured

percentage correct

Interview

Total

correct

Interview

number of errors

Person

M

SD

7.5

4.7

8.7

5.5

3.4*

3.0

5.7

3.7

6.6

4.4

1.9

1.5

Action

M

SD

6.2**

4.0

22.7

14.9

1.7

1.8

3.1

2.8

11.5

10.2

1.1

1.9

Object

M

SD

8.6**

4.3

10.8

5.4

1.8

1.9

5.3

3.8

6.7

4.8

1.0

1.4

Surrounding

M

SD

1.6

2.8

0.55

0.95

0.0

0.0

2.8

4.5

1.0

1.5

0.0

0.0

* p<. 05

** p <.01

Note. The 'percentage correct' measure shows the % of possible accurate recall for event

The person category was of most interest in the present study as it is known that both adults (MacLeod, Frowley, & Shepherd, 1994) and children (see Davies et al., 1989) have difficulty in describing people accurately (Clifford & Bull, 1978). An ANOVA revealed a significant effect of interview type on person errors in the QU phase (F(1, 59) = 6.38, p = .01) with the CI having significantly more person errors in this phase. Contrary to prediction, there were no effects of interview type for person correct (F(1, 59) = 2.65, p > .05).

Taking action details next, the CI was found to increase percentage correct information (F(1, 59) = 11.77, p<.001) but not action errors (F(1, 59) = 1.89).

Finally, for objects there was an effect of interview type on percentage correct in the QU phase (F=(1, 59) = 5.40, p < .01) with more correct information in the CI. There was no significant effect for object errors (F (1, 59)= 3.37, p = .07) or details about surroundings (F<1) across conditions.

Number of interviews and event knowledge

The 8 interviewers (4 cognitive and 4 structured) conducted between 5-10 interviews at Time 1 and 9-12 interviews each at Time 2. As the interviewers were unfamiliar with the event when they started interviewing at Time 1, it was important to check that they were not eliciting more information at the end of their series of interviews than at the beginning, due to their increased knowledge of the event. For each interviewer, interview position (first interview, second interview etc.) was correlated with amount of accurate information obtained. There were no significant correlations (Interviewer A, Fisher's r (5) = -.09; Interviewer B, r (8) =-.16; Interviewer C, r (6) =.24, Interviewer D, r (9) =.16, Interviewer E, r (10) =.10; Interviewer F, r (8) =.34, Interviewer G, r (6) =.24; Interviewer H, r (7) = -.09).

To summarise the results of the first interview those children having a CI interview at Time 1 recalled more correct information than did those having an SI. They also made more errors. However, accuracy rates did not differ. The significant effects occurred in the QU phase and examination of performance in this phase revealed that in the CI there was more correct recall about actions and objects and more person errors.

Second Interviews.

When analysing the data yielded at Time 2 a series of 2 x 3 ANOVAs were performed to examine CI-SI differences at Time 2 taking into account type of interview at Time 1 (CI/SI/None).

Amount Recalled at Time 2 Examination of percentage correct, amount of errors and amount of confabulations revealed a significant effect of interview type on percentage correct information only (F(2, 81) = 5.38, p <.01). Post-hoc analyses revealed that those children who had either a CI (Fisher's PLSD= 3.10, p < .05) or an SI (Fisher's PLSD = 4.43, p < .01) at Time 1 recalled significantly higher percentage correct at Time 2 than those children who were not interviewed at Time 1 (see Table 3 for means). There were no significant differences in accuracy of recall. The mean accuracy rate (across all conditions) was 84%.

Table 3: Percentage correct information, errors and confabulations means at Time 2.




Time 1




CI
SI
NONE



percent correct
total correct
percent correct
total correct
percent correct
total correct
Time 2
CI
M
16.4
105.4
18.1
115.7
13.5
86.5


SD
4.0
26.5
6.9
44.5
6.7
42.8

SI
M
15.6
100.5
16.3
104.5
12.2
78.2


SD
5.5
35.3
4.3
28.2
3.2
20.1

(ii) Errors


CI

SI
NONE
CI
M
16.2
12.2
11.2

SD
7.9
7.6
6.9
SI
M
15.0
16.3
11.3

SD
8.5
12.4
11.0

(iii) Confabulations


CI

SI
NONE
CI
M
6.8
7.4
4.1

SD
10.3
6.8
4.6
SI
M
5.7
8.4
9.3

SD
7.1
19.4
13.0

Note. The 'percentage correct' measure shows the % of possible accurate recall for event. Due to dropouts at Time 2, the N for CI and SI at Time 1 were 26 and 28.

Having identified an increase in percentage correct information in the groups who experienced two interviews, analyses were performed to look at differences at each stage of their second interview (as with the Time 1 data).

Recall at each Phase of the Interview (Time 2) When a series of 3 x 2 ANOVAs was performed on the data, with percentage correct, errors, and confabulations at each phase of the interview as the dependent measures, the following results were obtained. Taking the FR phase first, ANOVA revealed a significant effect of first interview on percentage correct free recall (F (2,81)= 7.06, p<.01). Post-hoc analyses showed that the children who had no interview at Time 1 recalled a lower percentage of correct information during the free report phase of their interview at Time 2 than those who had either a CI (Fisher's PLSD = 3.73, p < .01) or an SI (Fisher's PLSD = .25, p < .01) at Time 1. No other significant effects were noted in the FR phase.

In the QU phase, there was an effect of first interview on percentage correct (F (2,81)= 4.11, p<.05). Contrary to prediction, those children having an SI interview at Time 1 reported more correct information in the QU phase at Time 2 compared to those having a CI interview at Time 1 (Fisher's PLSD = 1.82, p=.01) and compared to those having no interview at Time 1 (Fisher's PLSD = 1.44, p<.05). There were no other significant effects in the QU phase and no significant effects in the SR phase.

Proportion of new and repeated information at Time 2 The coding scheme developed for the second interviews enabled the information to be categorised as either new (not mentioned at Time 1) or repeated information (mentioned at Time 1 and repeated at Time 2). For those children having two interviews (n = 54), the amount of repeated and new information and the proportion of their recall that was either new or repeated were noted. The amount of new and repeated information did not vary as a function of interview method. Interestingly at Time 2, 59% of accurate information was repeated and 41% was new; 23% of errors were repeated and 77% were new; 12% of confabulations were repeated and 88% were new. There are many possible reasons for the new information at Time 2 (including the children having talked amongst themselves and to others between interviews). It is worthy of note that a number of script relevant intrusions occurred at the second interview and this may reflect the influence of schema on gaps in memory (see Milne et al, 1995; Wark, Memon, Koehnken and Bull, 1995 for specific examples).

Number of questions asked and recall performance. Both groups of interviewers (CI and SI) were instructed to ask questions concerning only what the children had told them during the free report phase of the interview. As stated above, no differences in the amount recalled during the free report phase were found between those children having a CI and those having an SI at Time 1 or Time 2. However, in this phase the CI interviewers asked significantly more questions at Time 1 than did the SI interviewers (t(58) = 8.06, p < .0001, means for CI = 30.18 and for the SI = 12.88). Of these questions 66% were open questions, 22% closed and the remaining 2% fell into the leading or multiple choice category. The same pattern was found at Time 2 but here there were no CI/SI differences. The greater number of questions asked by CI interviewers suggests that when faced with the same amount of free recall information as their SI counterparts the CI interviewers subsequently adopted a more detailed questioning strategy.

Discussion

At Time 1 (two day delay) the present study found the CI to produce an increase in the amount of correct recall. The effect size (d =.58) was somewhat smaller than that reported in earlier studies (see Koehnken et al., 1994) but the control group (the structured interview) is superior to that used in the majority of previous studies. The increase in correct recall with the CI emanated from significantly more accurate object and action details. The increases in correct recall and errors were manifested in the questioning phase of the interview and this raises a number of questions about the effects of techniques used in this stage of the interview.

To what extent are the increase in reporting of details in the questioning phase of the CI due to the use of specific CI techniques? The CI interviewers used a combination of context reinstatement and imagery instructions in the questioning phase. The interviewers actively encouraged the children to generate images of the event and to describe them. They prompted the children with specific questions based on the information the children had given in free recall. The two techniques, activation of images and detailed questioning could account for the gains in correct and incorrect details. While imagery may be used to facilitate retrieval of information from memory, it may also increase inaccurate reporting as can be seen in the literature on suggestibility and creation of false memories (Ceci, Loftus, Leichtman and Bruck, 1994; Hyman and Pentland, 1995). Similarly, the use of specific questions with children of the age group sampled in the current study may increase the amount of information that is reported but at the cost of a drop in accuracy (Davies et al, 1989; Dent & Stephenson, 1979; King & Yuille, 1987; Oates and Shrimpton, 1991).

If we look at the absolute numbers of errors compared to correct details, it is clear that the CI produced a greater increase in correct details than in incorrect details. One plausible explanation for this is that the CI technique (which includes `report everything') shifts the criterion for responding by influencing confidence and willingness to report information (the theoretical basis is illustrated by signal detection theory). Bekerian and Dennett (1993) drew attention to this in a review paper which assessed the theoretical basis of CI. The effect of CI on the response criterion requires further investigation. It is possible that the errors are a result of demand characteristics or social pressure to give a desired response (Cronin et al, 1992). Asking probing questions can have this effect (Davies et al, 1989; Dent, 1991; Oates and Shrimpton, 1991). This study was, however, able to locate the nature of the errors that were being made. The children made more errors about persons than any other type of information. Again previous literature suggests that children have difficulty in describing persons (Davies et al., 1989; Gee & Pipe, 1995).

How should the increase in errors be interpreted by practitioners? First of all it should be noted that the CI increases errors but not confabulated details. An increase in confabulated details (e.g. saying the magician wore a cloak when there was no cloak) may have quite different ramifications in a forensic context than a person error (e.g. describing a purple jacket as a blue one). Secondly, if we look at the absolute number of correct details compared to errors in the present study, for every six correct details, there was an error, so the gains appear to outweigh the risks. Thirdly, there is no difference in accuracy rates across cognitive and structured interview conditions. While procedures such as the CI aim to increase the amount of information that is reported, in a forensic context it is the accuracy of the information that is crucial (Koriat and Goldsmith, 1994). In a recent review, Koriat and Goldsmith (in press) demonstrate how critical it is to separate the measure of quantity of information which fits the `storehouse metaphor' of traditional research from applied research on everyday memory processes. We suggest that the CI be used with older children (Saywitz et al, 1992) but some caution be exercised when interviewing younger children. Whenever possible external corroboration should be sought for the details reported. Special care should be taken in eliciting person descriptions as several studies have found errors with this type of information in a CI (see also Milne and Bull, 1995; Milne et al, 1995). Finally, it may be the case that the effects obtained here are specific to the 8-9 year old age group. We would encourage researchers to test the effectiveness of the CI procedure described in this study with older and younger children.

While the Time 1 results were consistent with our hypotheses, the Time 2 results were not. In light of the evidence that the CI (at Time 1 and Time 2) occasions a more extensive memory search, we predicted an increase in additional and repeated information in the CI/CI group at Time 2. Similarly, we predicted a carryover effect boosting performance of the CI/SI group. This was not found; as there was no effect of CI at Time 2 (although performance was better if children had been interviewed at Time 1). A number of recent studies have obtained similar results (Brock and Fisher, 1994; McCauley and Fisher, 1995). There are several possibilities as to why this may be the case. Taking the position that CI achieves its effect by enhancing interviewer communication and interviewee expectations, perhaps a good first (SI) interview is sufficient to raise performance levels (Roediger and Payne, 1982). It is possible that over the delay memory for the event became less context dependent (c.f., Smith, 1988). This would not be incompatible with recent theoretical accounts of memory and forgetting (e.g., Brainerd et al., 1990; Roediger & Challis, 1989; Riccio, Rabinowitz & Axelrod, 1994). Similarly, Payne et al. (1993) maintain that context is a poorer cue at the delayed test than at immediate test either as a consequence of a decrement in context-item association strength, or as a result of changes in the functional context between the two sessions.

The improvements following repeated testing suggest a powerful effect of retrieval practice. If a retrieval attempt in first interview serves to strengthen item-to-context associations sufficiently, such associations would tend to be recovered on the second test (Brainerd et al., 1990). As indicated in the introduction, it is well established in the memory literature that testing shortly after exposure to the to-be-remembered material will attenuate forgetting over a delay (e.g., Brainerd & Ornstein, 1991, for a review). The extent to which quality of the first interview may determine the amount reported upon repeated testing may be explored in future studies by including an untrained control group.

Further research could also be usefully deployed to look at conditions under which repeated interviews are most likely to be effective. From a practical and theoretical perspective, it is important to understand how performance with a CI will vary with longer delays, to examine the patterns of losses and gains (e.g., the frequency with which repeated and new information is reported), and factors that may reduce inter-test forgetting. Interviewer credibility, demand characteristics, interviewer instructions, and an interviewee's interpretation of the interviewer's requests may also interact in interesting ways with interview technique and these may account for changes in response criteria across repeated tests. Memory theorists have recently developed experimental procedures (e.g., the `logic of opposition' procedure see Jacoby, Woloshyn, & Kelley, 1989; Lindsay, Gonzales, & Eso, 1995) which can be applied to address these issues.

References

Bekerian, D.A. & Dennett, J.L. (1993). The cognitive interview: Reviving the issues. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 7 (4), 275-298.

Boggs, S.R., & Eyberg, S. (1990). Interview techniques and establishing rapport. In A.M. La Greca (Ed.), Through the eyes of the child: Obtaining self-reports from children and adolescents (pp. 85-108). Allyn and Bacon: Boston.

Brainerd, C., & Ornstein, P.A. (1991). Children's memory for witnessed events. In J. Doris (Ed.), The suggestibility of children's recollections. (pp. 10-20). American Psychological Association: Washington D.C.

Brainerd, C.J., Reyna, V.F., Howe, M.L., & Kingma, J. (1990). The development of forgetting and reminiscence. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 55, (3-4 Serial No 222).

Brock, P & Fisher, R.P. (1994, March). Effectiveness of the cognitive interview in a multiple testing session. Paper presented at the Biennial Conference of the American Psychology Law Society, Sante Fe.

Bull, R. (1995a) Interviewing children in legal contexts. In R.Bull and D. Carson (Eds.), Handbook of psychology in legal contexts, (pp. 235-246). Chichester, Wiley.

Bull, R. (1995b). Innovative techniques for the questioning of child witnesses especially those who are young and those with learning disability. In M. Zaragoza, J.R. Graham, G.C.N. Hall, R. Hirschman & Y.S. Ben-Porath (Eds.), Memory and testimony in the child witness (pp. 179-194). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Bull, R. (1992). Obtaining evidence expertly: The reliability of interviews with child witnesses. Expert Evidence: The International Digest of Human Behaviour, Science and Law, 1 (1), 3-36.

Ceci, S., Loftus, E., Leichtman, M. and Bruck, M. (1994) The possible role of source misattributions in the creation of false beliefs among preschoolers. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 42, 304-20.

Cole, D. A., Maxwell, S.E., Arvey, R. & Salas, E. (1994). How the power of a MANOVA can both increase and decrease as a function of intercorrelations among the dependent variables. Psychological Bulletin, 115 (3), 465-74.

Clifford, B. & Bull, R. (1978). The psychology of person identification London: Routledge.

Clyde, J. (1992). The report of the inquiry into the removal of children from Orkney in February 1991. Edinburgh: HMSO.

Cronin, O., Memon, A., Eaves, R., Kupper, B. & Bull, R. (1992, May). The cognitive interview with child witnesses: a child centered approach? Paper presented at NATO Advanced Study Institute: The child witness in context. Italy.

Davies, G., Tarrant, A., & Flin, R. (1989) . Close encounters of the witness kind: children's memory for a simulated health inspection. British Journal of Psychology, 80, 415-429.

Dent, H.R. (1991). Experimental studies of interviewing child witnesses. In J. Doris (Ed.) The suggestibility of children's recollections (pp. 138-146). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Dent, H.R. & Stephenson, G.M. (1979). An experimental study of the effectiveness of different techniques of questioning child witnesses. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 18, 41-51.

Eich, J.E. (1980). The cue dependent nature of state dependent retrieval. Memory and Cognition, 8, 157-173.

Fisher, R.P. & Geiselman, R.E. (1992). Memory enhancing techniques for investigative interviewing: The Cognitive Interview. Springfield III.: Charles C. Thomas.

Fisher, R.P., Geiselman, R.E., Raymond, D.S., Jurkevich, L.M. & Warhaftig, M.L. (1987). Enhancing eyewitness memory: refining the cognitive interview. Journal of Police Science and Administration, 15, 291-297.

Flavell, J.H. & Wellman, E. (1977). Metamemory. In R.V. Kail and J. W. Hagen (Eds.), Perspectives on the development of memory and cognition (pp. 3-33). Hillsdale N.J: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Flin, R., Boon, J,. Knox, A., & Bull, R. (1992). The effects of a five month delay on children's eyewitness memory. British Journal of Psychology, 83, 323-336.

Gee, S., & Pipe, M-E. (1995). Helping children to remember: The influence of object cues on children's accounts of a real event. Developmental Psychology, 31, (5), 746-58.

Geiselman, R.E., & Fisher, R.P. (1988). The cognitive interview: An innovative technique for questioning witnesses of crime. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 2 (2), 2-5.

Geiselman, R.E., Fisher, R.P., MacKinnon, D.P., & Holland, H.L. (1985). Eyewitness memory enhancement in the police interview: Cognitive retrieval mnemonics versus hypnosis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 70, 401-412.

Geiselman, R.E., Fisher, R.P., MacKinnon, D.P., & Holland, H.L. (1986). Eyewitness memory enhancement in the cognitive interview. American Journal of Psychology, 99, 385-401.

Goodman, G. & Bottoms, B. (1993). Child victims, child witnesses: Understanding and Improving testimony. New York: The Guilford Press.

Goodman, G.S., Aman, C., & Hirschman, J. (1987). Child sexual and physical abuse: Children's testimony. In S.J. Ceci, M.P. Toglia, & D.F. Ross (Eds.), Children's eyewitness memory (pp.1-23). New York: Springer-Verlag.

Hudson, J.A. (1990) Constructive processes in children's event memory. Developmental Psychology, 26, 180-186.

Hyman, I.E. and Pentland, J. (1995) The role of imagery in the creation of false childhood memories. Manuscript submitted for publication.

Jacoby, L.L., Woloshyn, V., & Kelley, C.M. (1989). Becoming famous without being recognised: Unconscious influences of memory produced by dividing attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 115-125.

King, M., & Yuille, J. (1987). Suggestibility and the child witness. In S.J. Ceci, M. Toglia, & D. Ross (Eds.), Children's eyewitness memory (pp.24-35). New York: Springer-Verlag.

Koehnken, G., Finger, M., Nitschke, N., Höfer, E. & Aschermann, E. (1992). Does a cognitive interview interfere with a subsequent Statement Validity Analysis? Paper presented at the American Psychology and Law Society meeting, San Diego.

Koehnken, G. Milne, R. Memon, A. & Bull, R. (1994, March). A meta-analysis of the effects of the Cognitive Interview. Paper presented at the Biennial Conference of the American Psychology Law Society, Sante Fe.

Koehnken, G., Schimmossek, E., Aschermann, E. and Höfer (in press). The cognitive interview and the assessment of the credibility of adults' statements. Journal of Applied Psychology

Koehnken, G., Thuerer, C. & Zorberbier, D. (1994). The cognitive interview: Are interviewers' memories enhanced too? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 8, 13-24.

Koriat, A. & Goldsmith, M. (in press). Memory metaphors and the everyday laboratory controversy: Comparing the storehouse and correspondence conceptions of memory. Brain and Behavioural Sciences.

Koriat, A. & Goldsmith, M. (1994). Memory in naturalistic and laboratory contexts: distinguishing accuracy oriented and quantity oriented approaches to memory assessment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123, 397-315.

Lindsay, D.S., Gonzales, V. & Eso, K. (1995). Aware and unaware uses of memories of postevent suggestions. In M. Zaragoza, J.R. Graham, G.C.N. Hall, R. Hirschman & Y.S. Ben-Porath (Eds.), Memory and testimony in the child witness (pp. 86-108). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage

Loftus, E.F. and Fathi, D.C. (1985). Retrieving multiple autobiographical memories. Social Cognition, 3, 280-95.

MacLeod, M., Frowley, J., & Shepherd, J. (1994). Whole body information and its relevance to eyewitness identification. In D. Ross, J.D. Read, & M. Toglia (Eds.), Adult Eyewitness Testimony: Current Trends and Developments (pp. 125-143). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

Mantwill, M., Koehnken, G., & Aschermann, E. (1995). Effects of the cognitive interview on the recall of familiar and unfamiliar events. Journal of Applied Psychology, 80, 68-78.

McCauley, M.R. & Fisher, R.P. (1995) Facilitating children's eyewitness recall with the revised cognitive interview. Journal of Applied Psychology, 80, (4), 510-517.

Memon, A., & Bruce, V. (1985). Context effects in episodic studies of verbal and facial memory: a review, Current Psychological Research and Reviews, Winter 1985-86, 349-369.

Memon, A. Cronin, Ó, Eaves, R. and Bull, R. (in press). An empirical test of the mnemonic components of the Cognitive Interview. In G.M. Davies, S. Lloyd-Bostock, M. McMurran & C. Wilson (Eds.), Psychology and Law: Advances in Research. Berlin: De Gruyter.

Memon, A, Cronin, Ó, Eaves, R. & Bull, R. (1993) The Cognitive Interview and Child Witnesses. In G.M. Stephenson & N.K. Clark (Eds.) Children, Evidence and Procedure. Issues in Criminological & Legal Psychology. No. 20. (pp. 3-9). British Psychological Society, Leicester, UK.

Memorandum of Good Practice (1992). Department of Home and Health. London: HMSO.

Milne, R. & Bull, R. (1995, September). Interviewing children with mild learning difficulty with the cognitive interview. Paper presented at the Division of Criminological & Legal Psychology Annual Conference, Rugby.

Milne, R., Bull, R., Koehnken, G. & Memon, A. (1995) The cognitive interview and suggestibility. In G.M. Stephenson & N.K. Clark (Eds.), Criminal Behaviour: Perceptions, Attributions and Rationality. Division of Criminological & Legal Psychology Occasional Papers, No. 22. (pp. 21-27). British Psychological Society, Leicester, UK.

Oates, K., & Shrimpton, S. (1991). Children's memories for stressful and non-stressful events. Medical Science and Law, 31, 4-10.

Paivio, A. (1971). Imagery and verbal processes. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.

Payne, D.G. (1987). Hypermnesia and reminiscence in recall: A historical and empirical review. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 5-27.

Payne, D.G., Hembrooke, H. A. & Anastasi, J.S. (1993). Hypermnesia in free and cued recall. Memory and Cognition, 21, 48-62.

Poole, D.A., & White, L.T. (1995). Tell me again and again: stability and change in repeated testimonies of children and adults. In M. Zaragoza, J.R. Graham, G.C.N. Hall, R. Hirschman & Y.S. Ben-Porath (Eds.), Memory and testimony in the child witness (pp. 24-43). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Poole, D.A., & White, L.T. (1993) Two years later: Effects of question repetition and retention interval on the eyewitness testimony of children and adults. Developmental Psychology, 27, 975-86.

Riccio, D.C. Rabinowitz, V.C. & Axelrod, S. (1994). Memory: when less is more. American Psychologist, 49, (11), 917-926.

Roediger, H.L. & Payne, D.G. (1982) Hypermnesia: The role of repeated testing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: learning, memory and cognition. 8, 66-72.

Roediger, H.L., III, & Challis, B.H. (1989). Hypermnesia: Improvements in recall with repeated testing. In C. Z. Izawa (Ed.), Current issues in cognitive processes: The Tulane Floweree symposium on cognition (pp. 175-199). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Saywitz, K.J., Geiselman, R.E. & Bornstein, G.K. (1992). Effects of cognitive interviewing and practice on children's recall performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 77, 744-756

Scrivner, E., & Safer, M.A. (1988). Eyewitnesses show hypermnesia for details about a violent event. Journal of Applied Psychology, 73, 371-377.

Smith, S (1988). Environmental context dependent memory. In G. Davies and D. Thomson (Eds.) Memory in context: Context in memory. (pp. 13-34). London: John Wiley & sons.

Tulving, E., & Thomson, D.M. (1973). Encoding specificity and retrieval processes in episodic memory. Psychological Review, 80, 353-370.

Turtle, J. & Yuille, J. (1994). Lost but not forgotten details: Repeated eyewitness recall leads to reminiscence but not hypermnesia. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 260-71.

Wark, L., Memon, A., Koehnken, G. & Bull, R. (1995) Uses and abuses of schematic knowledge. University of Southampton, Department of Psychology, Working Paper, RWPS/95/1

Warren, A. & Lane, P. (1995). Effects of timing and type of questioning on eyewitness accuracy and suggestibility. In M. Zaragoza, J.R. Graham, G.C.N. Hall, R. Hirschman & Y.S. Ben-Porath (Eds.), Memory and testimony in the child witness (pp. 44-60). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by a grant from the British Economic and Social Research Council (ESRC) R000234290. We are grateful to the staff and pupils of Bassett Green First and Middle School. We thank Angela Holley and Rebecca Milne for their contribution to the research; our interviewers for data collection; Tony Roberts and Chris Colbourn for statistical advice, and Anne Anderson, Ed Geiselman, Mel Pipe, Debra Poole, Steve Lindsay and Sarah Stevenage for their comments on earlier drafts of the paper.

[1]Of the 109 children, 9 children were absent for testing; In addition, two interviews (one SI, one CI) were discarded without coding on the basis that the children had made no attempt to talk about the event. One further interview (CI) was dropped from the sample because it appeared as an outlier with an extremely unusual number of confabulations (n=78). Several CI/ SI interviews from Time 1 and Time 2 were not transcribed as the tape recordings were of poor sound quality. This reduced the total number of interviews used in the analyses to 87.

[2]MANOVAS were not performed on the data as the dependent variables did not always correlate (see Cole, Maxwell, Arvey and Salas, 1994).

[3] Combining all the phases in one repeated measures ANOVA was deemed inappropriate as each phase involved different recall techniques, and recall at each phase was scored on the basis of what was recalled at the earlier phases (additional information). The same principle was adhered to regarding detail types.