Cogprints

Rethinking the ontogeny of mindreading

Tirassa, Maurizio and Bosco, Francesca M. and Colle, Livia (2006) Rethinking the ontogeny of mindreading. [Journal (Paginated)]

Full text available as:

[img]
Preview
PDF
200Kb

Abstract

We propose a mentalistic and nativist view of human early mental and social life and of the ontogeny of mindreading. We define the mental state of sharedness as the primitive, one-sided capability to take one's own mental states as mutually known to an i nteractant. We argue that this capability is an innate feature of the human mind, which the child uses to make a subjective sense of the world and of her actions. We argue that the child takes all of her mental states as shared with her caregivers. This a llows her to interact with her caregivers in a mentalistic way from the very beginning and provides the grounds on which the later maturation of mindreading will build. As the latter process occurs, the child begins to understand the mental world in terms of differences between the mental states of different agents; subjectively, this also corresponds to the birth of privateness. ˇ

Item Type:Journal (Paginated)
Keywords:Keywords: Mindreading; Theory of mind; Communication; Consciousness; Subjectivity; Sharedness; Privateness; Social cognition; Nativism
Subjects:Neuroscience > Neuropsychology
Psychology > Developmental Psychology
Philosophy > Philosophy of Language
Biology > Primatology
Psychology > Comparative Psychology
Psychology > Cognitive Psychology
Philosophy > Philosophy of Mind
Psychology > Evolutionary Psychology
Psychology > Clinical Psychology
Linguistics > Pragmatics
Psychology > Social Psychology
Philosophy > Epistemology
ID Code:5214
Deposited By: Tirassa, Prof. Maurizio
Deposited On:09 Oct 2006
Last Modified:11 Mar 2011 08:56

References in Article

Select the SEEK icon to attempt to find the referenced article. If it does not appear to be in cogprints you will be forwarded to the paracite service. Poorly formated references will probably not work.

Airenti, G., Bara, B. G., & Colombetti, M. (1993). Conversation and behaviour games in the pragmatics of dialogue. Cognitive Science, 17, 197-256.

Baillargeon, R. (1995). Physical reasoning in infants. In A. J. Premack, D. Premack, & D. Sperber (Eds.), C ausal cognition: A multidisciplinary debate. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (Eds.). (1992). The adapted mind. Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. New York and Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Baron-C ohen, S. (1993). From attention-goal psychology to belief-desire psychology: The development of theory of mind and its dysfunction. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism. Oxford: Ox ford University Press.

Baron-Cohen, S. (1994). How to build a baby that can read minds: Cognitive mechanisms in mindreading. Current Psychology of Cognition, 13, 513-552.

Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Barresi, J., & M oore, C. (1996). Intentional relations and social understanding. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 19, 107-154.

Bartsch, K., & Wellman, H. M. (1995). Children talk about the mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Cam aioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1979). Cognition and communication from 9-13 months. In E. Bates (Ed.), The emergence of symbols. New York: Academic.

Bermudez, J. L. (1998). The paradox of self-consciousness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Bosco, F. M., & Tirassa, M. (1998). Sharedness as an innate basis for communication in the infant. Proceedings of the 20th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: I - Attachment. London: Hogarth.

Braten, S. (1992). The virtual other in infant's minds and social feelings. In H. Wold (Ed.), The dialogical alternative (pp. 77-97). Oslo: Scandinavian University Press.

Brazetlon, T. G., Koslowsky, B., & Main, M. (1974). The origins of reciprocity: The early mother-infant interaction. In M. Lewis & L. Rosenblum (Eds.), The effect of the infant on its caregivers. New York: Wiley.

Brown, R., Hobson, A. L., & Stevenson, J. (1997). Are there 'autistic-like' features in congenitally blind children. Journal of Chi ld Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 693-704.

Butterworth, G. (1991). The ontogeny and phylogeny of joint visual attention. In A. Whiten (Ed.), Natural theories of the mind. Oxford: Blackwell.

Carassa, A., Morganti, F., & Tirassa, M. (2004). Movement, acti on, and situation. In M. Alcaniz Raya & B. Rey Solaz (Eds.), Proceedings of the 7th annual international workshop on presence. Valencia, Spain: Editorial Universidad Politecnica de Valencia.

Carassa, A., Morganti, F., & Tirassa, M. (2005). A situated cog nition perspective on presence. Proceedings of the 27th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Stresa, Italy.

Carey, S. (1995). On the origin of causal understanding. In A. James Premack, D. Premack, & D. Sperber (Eds.), Causal cognition. Oxford: Clarendon.

Charlesworth, W. R., & Kreutzer, M. A. (1973). An ethological approach to research on facial expression. In P. Ekman (Ed.), Darwin and facial expression. New York: Academic Press.

Chomsky, N. (1980). Rules and representations. New York: Columbia University Press.

Clark, H. H., & Marshall, C. R. (1981). Definite reference and mutual knowledge. In A. K. Joshi, B. L. Webber, & I. A. Sag (Eds.), Elements of discourse understanding. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Colombetti, M. (19 93). Formal semantics for mutual belief. Artificial Intelligence, 62, 341-353.

Dunn, J., Brown, J., Slomkowski, C., Tesla, C., & Youngblade, L. (1991). Young children's understanding of other people's feelings and beliefs: Individual differences and thei r antecedents. Child Development, 62, 1352-1366.

Edelman, G. M. (1992). Bright air, brilliant fire: On the matter of the mind. New York: Basic Books.

Flavell, J. H., Everett, B. A., Croft, K., & Flavell, E. R. (1981). Young children's knowledge about vi sual perception. Developmental Psychology, 17, 99-103.

Fodor, J. A. (1975). The language of thought. New York: Crowell.

Fodor, J. A. (1981). The present status of the innateness controversy. In Representations. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Fodor, J. A. (1 987). Psychosemantics. The problem of meaning in the philosophy of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Fodor, J. A. (1992). A theory of the child's theory of mind. Cognition, 44, 283-296.

Fodor, J. A. (1998). Concepts. Where cognitive science went wrong. Ox ford: Oxford University Press.

Freud, S. (1911). Formulation on the two principles of mental functioning. In J. Strachey (Ed.), Sigmund Freud, (Vol. 12, pp. 213-216). London: Hogarth Press.

Gallese, V., & Goldman, A. (1998). Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mindreading. Trends in Cognitive Science, 2, 493-501.

Garfield, J. L., Peterson, C. C., & Perry, T. (2001). Social cognition, language acquisition and the development of the theory of mind. Mind and Language, 16, 494-541.

Gergely, G., & Watson, J. (1996). The social biofeedback model of parental affect-mirroring. International Journal of Psycho-Analysis, 77, 1181-1212.

Gergely, G., & Watson, J. (1999). Early social-emotional development: Contingency perception and the social biofeedback model. In P. Rochat (Ed.), Early social cognition: Understanding others in the first months of life (pp. 101-137). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Goldman, A. (1993). The psychology of folk psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 15-28.

Gopnik, A. (1993). How we know our minds. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 1-14.

Gordon, R. M. (1996). Radical simulationism. In P. Carruthers & P. K. Smith (Eds.), Theories of theories of mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Grice, H. P. (1989). Studies in the way of words. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Hains, S. M. J., & Muir, D. W. (1996). Infant sensitivity to adult eye direction. Child Development, 67, 1940-1949.

Harris, P. L. (1991). The work of the imagination. In A. Whiten (Ed.), Natural theories of the mind. Oxford: Blackwell.

Haviland, J. M., & Lelwica, M. (1987). The induced effect response. Developmental Psychology, 23, 97-104.

Hobson, R. P. (1993). Autism and the development of mind. Hove, UK: Erlbaum.

Lempers, J. D., Fla vell, E. R., & Flavell, J. H. (1977). The development in very young children of tacit knowledge concerning visual perception. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 95, 3-53.

Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretense and representation: The origins of theory of mind. Psych ological Review, 94, 412-426.

Leslie, A. M. (1994a). ToMM, ToBy, and agency. In L. Hirschfeld & S. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Leslie, A. M. (1994b). Pretending and believing: Issues in the theory of ToMM. Cogn ition, 50, 211-238.

Lewis, C., Freeman, N., Kyriakidou, C., Maridaki-Kassotaki, K., & Berridge, D. (1996). Social influences on false belief access: Specific sibling influences or general apprenticeship? Child Development, 67, 2930-2947.

Lewontin, R. (2 000). The triple helix: Gene, organism and environment. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Lorenz, K. (1965). Evolution and modification of behavior. Chicago and London: The University of Chicago Press.

Meltzoff, A. N., & Gopnik, A. (1993). The ro le of imitation in understanding persons and developing theories of mind. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1983). Newborn infants imitate adult facial gestures. Child Development, 54, 702-709.

Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1989). Imitation in newborn infants: Exploring the range of gestures imitated and the underlying mechanisms. Developmental Psychology, 25, 954-962.

Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1997). Explaining facial imitation: Theoretical model. Early Development and Parenting, 6, 179-192.

Moses, L. J., Baldwin, D. A., Rosicky, J. G., & Tidball, G. (2001). Evidence for referential understanding in the emotions domain at twel ve and eighteen months. Child Development, 72, 718-735.

Morton, J., & Johnson, M. H. (1991). CONSPECT and CONLEARN: A two process theory of infant face recognition. Psychological Review, 98, 164-181.

Murray, L., & Trevarthen, C. (1985). Emotional regula tion of interaction between 2-month-olds and their mother. In T. M. Field & N. A. Fox (Eds.), Social perception in infant. Norwood, NJ: Meredith.

Papousek, H., & Papousek, M. (1974). Mirror-image and self recognition in young infants: A new method of experimental analysis. Developmental Psychobiology, 7, 149-157.

Papousek, H., & Papousek, M. (1979). Early ontology of human social interaction: Its biological roots and social dimension. In M. von Cranach et al. (Eds.), Human ethology: Claim and limits of a new discipline. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Perner, J., Ruffman, T., & Leekam, S. (1994). Theory of mind is contagious: You can take it from your sibs. Child Development, 65, 1228-1238.

Peterson, C., & Siegal, M. (1995). Deafness, conversat ion and theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36, 459-474.

Piaget, J. (1936). La naissance de lintelligence chez lenfant. Neuchatel: Delachaux & Niestle. [Translation: The origins of intelligence in children. New York: International Universities Press, 1952].

Premack, D. (1990). The infant's theory of self-propelled objects. Cognition, 36, 1-16.

Premack, D., & James Premack, A. (1994). Why animals have neither culture nor history. In T. Ingold (Ed.), Companion encyclopedia of anth ropology. London: Routledge.

Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 512-526.

Repacholi, B. M., & Gopnik, A. (1997). Early reasoning about desires: Evidence from 14- and 18-month o lds. Developmental Psychology, 33, 12-21.

Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, V., & Fogassi, L. (1996). Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Cognitive Brain Research, 3, 131-141.

Rochat, P., & Morgan, R. (1995). Spatial determinants in the perception of self-produced leg movements in 3- to 5-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 31, 626-636.

Samuels, R. (2002). Nativism in cognitive science. Mind and Language, 17, 233-265.

Scaife, M., & Bruner, J. (1975). The capacity for join t visual attention in the infant. Nature, 235, 265-266.

Scholl, B. J., & Leslie, A. M. (1999). Modularity, development and theory of mind. Mind and Language, 14, 131-153.

Searle, J. R. (1983). Intentionality: An essay in the philosophy of mind. Cambridg e: Cambridge University Press.

Searle, J. R. (1992). The rediscovery of the mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Searle, J. R. (1995). The construction of social reality. New York: Free Press.

Spelke, E. (1994). Initial knowledge: Six suggestions. Cognition, 50, 431-445.

Spelke, E., & Van de Walle, G. (1993). Perceiving and reasoning about objects: Insigths from infants. In N. Eilan, W. Brewer, & R. McCarthy (Eds.), Spatial representation. New York: Basil Blackwell.

Stern, D. N. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant. New York: Basic Books.

Stern, D. N. (1995). Self/other differentiation in the domain of intimate socio-affective interaction: Some considerations. In P. Rochat (Ed.), The Self in infancy: Theory and research (pp. 419-429). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Stern, D. N., Jaffe, J., Beebe, B., & Bennett, S. K. (1975). Vocalizing in unison and alternation: Two modes of communication within the mother-infant dyad. In D. Aaronson & R. W. Rieber (Eds.), Developmental psycholinguistics and communicatio n disorders. New York: New York Academy of Sciences.

Thompson, R. (1998). Empathy and its origins in early development. In S. Braten (Ed.), Intersubjective communication and emotion in early ontogeny (pp. 144-157). Paris: Cambridge University Press.

Tir assa, M. (1997). Mental states in communication. Proceedings of the 2nd European Conference on Cognitive Science. Manchester, UK.

Tirassa, M. (1999a). Communicative competence and the architecture of the mind/brain. Brain and Language, 68, 419-441.

Tirassa, M. (1999b). Taking the trivial doctrine seriously: Functionalism, eliminativism, and materialism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 851-852.

Tirassa, M. (in press). Agencies. In: L. A. Perez Miranda & J. M. Larrazabal (Eds.), Advances in Cognitive Science. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

Tirassa, M., Carassa, A., & Geminiani, G. (2000). A theoretical framework for the study of spatial cognition. In S. O'Nuallain (Ed.), Spatial cognition. Foundations and applications. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: Benjamins.

Tomasel lo, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In C. Moore & P. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (in press). Understanding and sharing intentions: The ontogeny and phylogeny of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

Tomasello, M., & Rakoczy, H. (2003). What makes huma n cognition unique. From individual to shared to collective intentionality. Mind and Language, 8, 121-147.

Trevarthen, C. (1977). Descriptive analyses of infant communicative behavior. In H. Schaeffer (Ed.), Determinants of infant behavior. London: Acade mic.

Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and cooperation in early infancy. In M. Bullowa (Ed.), Before speech. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Trevarthen, C. (1993). The function of emotions in early infant communication and development. In J. N adel & L. Camaioni (Eds.), New perspectives in early communicative development (pp. 48-81). New York: Routledge.

Trevarthen, C. (1998). The concept and foundations of infant intersubjectivity. In S. Braten (Ed.), Intersubjectivity communication and emoti on in early ontogeny (pp. 15-46). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Trevarthen, C. (2001). The neurobiology of early communication: Intersubjective regulations in human brain development. In A. F. Kalverboer & A. Gramsbergen (Eds.), Handbook on brai n and behavior in human development. Dordrecht, The Neverlands: Klewer Academic Publisher.

Trevarthen, C., & Aitken, K. J. (2001). Infant intersubjectivity: Research, theory and clinical applications. Journal of Psychological Psychiatry, 42, 3-48.

Troni ck, E., Als, H., & Adamson, L. (1979). Structure of early face-to-face communicative interactions. In M. Bullowa (Ed.), Before speech. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Varela, F. J., Thompson, E., & Rosch, E. (1991). The embodied mind. Cognitive sci ence and human experience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Wellman, H. M. (1990). The child's theory of mind. Cambridge: MIT Press.

Wellman, H. M. (1991). From desires to beliefs: Acquisition of a theory of mind. In A. Whiten (Ed.), Natural theories of the mind. Oxford: Blackwell.

Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72, 655-684.

Whiten, A. (1994). Grades of mindreading. In C. Lewis & P. Mitchell (Eds.), Children's early understanding of the mind: Origins and development. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs. Representation and constraining functions of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. C ognition, 13, 103-128.

Woodward, A. (1998). Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach. Cognition, 69, 1-34.

ia

Metadata

Repository Staff Only: item control page