Cogprints

Sharedness and privateness in human early social life

Tirassa, Maurizio and Bosco, Francesca M. and Colle, Livia (2006) Sharedness and privateness in human early social life. [Journal (Paginated)]

Full text available as:

[img]
Preview
PDF
206Kb

Abstract

This research is concerned with the innate predispositions underlying human intentional communication. Human communication is currently defined as a circular and overt attempt to modify a partner's mental states. This requires each party involved to posse ss the ability to represent and understand the other's mental states, a capability which is commonly referred to as mindreading, or theory of mind (ToM). The relevant experimental literature agrees that no such capability is to be found in the human speci es at least during the first year of life, and possibly later. This paper aims at advancing a solution to this theoretical problem. We propose to consider sharedness as the basis for intentional communication in the infant and to view it as a primitive, i nnate component of her cognitive architecture. Communication can then build upon the mental grounds that the infant takes as shared with her caregivers. We view this capability as a theory of mind in a weak sense.›

Item Type:Journal (Paginated)
Keywords:Communication; Mindreading; Theory of mind; Consciousness; Subjectivity; Sharedness; Privateness; Social cognition; Nativism
Subjects:Neuroscience > Neuropsychology
Psychology > Developmental Psychology
Philosophy > Philosophy of Language
Biology > Primatology
Psychology > Comparative Psychology
Psychology > Cognitive Psychology
Philosophy > Philosophy of Mind
Psychology > Evolutionary Psychology
Psychology > Clinical Psychology
Linguistics > Pragmatics
Psychology > Social Psychology
Philosophy > Epistemology
ID Code:5215
Deposited By: Tirassa, Prof. Maurizio
Deposited On:09 Oct 2006
Last Modified:11 Mar 2011 08:56

References in Article

Select the SEEK icon to attempt to find the referenced article. If it does not appear to be in cogprints you will be forwarded to the paracite service. Poorly formated references will probably not work.

Airenti, G. (1998). Dialogue in a developmental perspective. In S. Cmejrkova, J. Hoffmannova, O. Mullerova, & J. Svetla (Eds.), 6th Conference of the International Association for Dialogue Analysis (pp. 283-290). Tubingen: Niemeyer.

Airenti, G., Bara, B. G., & Colombetti, M. (1993). Conversation and behavior games in the pragmatics of dialogue. Cognitive Science, 17, 197-256.

Allen, J. F., & Perrault, C. R. (1980). Analyzing intention in utterances. Artificial Intelligence, 15, 143-178.

Austin, J. L. (1962). How to do things with words (2nd ed. revised by J. O. Ormson, & M. Sbisa, 1975). London: Oxford University Press.

Bara, B. G., & Tirassa, M. (1999). A mentalist framework for linguistic and extralinguistic communication. In S. Bagnara (Ed.), 3rd E uropean conference on cognitive science (pp. 285-290). Roma: IP-CNR.

Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (Eds.) (1992). The adapted mind. Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. New York and Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness. An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a theory of mind? Cognition, 21, 37-46.

Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1979). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. In E. Ochs & B. B. Schieffelin (Eds.), Developmental pragmatics (pp. 111-129). New York: Academic.

Bateson, G. (1972). Steps to an ecology of mind. New York: Ballantine.

Bosco, F. M., & Tiras sa, M. (1998). Sharedness as an innate basis for communication in the infant. In M. A. Gernsbacher & S. J. Derry (Eds.), 20th Annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 162-166). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: I - Attachment. London: Hogarth.

Bratman, M. E. (1987). Intentions, plans, and practical reason. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Bratman, M. E. (1990). What is intention? In P. R. Cohen, J. Morgan, & M. E. Pollack (Eds.), Intentions in communica t ion (pp. 15-31). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Braten, S. (1992). The virtual other in infants' minds and social feelings. In H. Wold (Ed.), The dialogical alternative (pp. 77-97). Oslo: Scandinavian University Press.

Bruner, J. S. (1977). Early social inte raction and language acquisition. In H. R. Schaffer (Ed.), Studies in mother-infant interaction (pp. 271-289). London: Academic Press.

Bruner, J. S. (1982). Formats of language acquisition. American Journal of Semiotics, 1, 1-16.

Bruner, J. S. (1990). A cts of meaning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Carassa, A., Morganti, F., & Tirassa, M. (2004). Movement, action, and situation: presence in virtual environments. In M.Alcaniz Raya & B. Rey Solaz (Eds.), 7th Annual international workshop on p resence (pp. 7-12). Valencia, Spain: Editorial Universidad Politecnica de Valencia.

Carassa, A., Morganti, F., & Tirassa, M. (2005). A situated cognition perspective on presence. 27th Annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 384-389). Mahw ah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Carey, S. (1995). On the origin of causal understanding. In A. James Premack, D. Premack, & D. Sperber (Eds.), Causal cognition (pp. 268-301). Oxford: Clarendon.

Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63 (4, Serial No. 255).

Clancey, W. J. (1997a). The conceptual nature of knowledge, situations and activity. In P. J. Feltov ich, K. M. Ford, & R. R. Hoffmann (Eds.), Expertise in context (pp. 247-291). Menlo Park, CA: The AAAI Press.

Clancey, W. J. (1997b). Situated cognition. On human knowledge and computer representations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Clark, H. H. (1 992). Arenas of language use. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Clark, H. H. (1996). Using language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Clark, H. H., & Marshall, C. R. (1981). Definite reference and mutual knowledge. In A. K. Joshi, B. L. Webber, & I. A. Sag (Eds.), Elements of discourse understanding (pp. 10-63). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Cohen, P. R., Morgan, J., & Pollack, M. E. (Eds.). (1990). Intentions in communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Cohen, P. R., & Perr ault, C. R. (1979). Elements of a plan-based theory of speech acts. Cognitive Science, 3, 177-212.

Cohn, J. F., & Tronik, E. Z. (1989). Specificity of infants' response to mothers' affective behavior. Journal of the American Academy of Child an Adoloscen t Psy chiatry, 28, 242-248.

Cole, M. (1996). Cultural psychology. A once and future discipline. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Colombetti, M. (1993). Formal semantics for mutual belief. Artificial Intelligence, 62, 341-353.

Cosmides, L., & To oby, J. (1994a). Beyond intuition and instinct blindness: Toward an evolutionarily rigorous cognitive science. Cognition, 50, 41-77.

Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1994b). Origins of domain specificity: The evolution of functional organization. In L. A. Hirs chfeld & S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind. Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 85-116). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Dennett, D. C. (1978). Beliefs about beliefs. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 568-570.

Dennett, D. C. (198 7). The intentional stance. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Edelman, G. M. (1992). Bright air, brilliant fire: on the matter of the mind. New York: Basic Books.

Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of mind. An essay on faculty psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Fodor, J. A. (2000). The mind doesn't work that way. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Fogel, A. (1993). Two principles of communication: co-regulation and framing. In J. Nadel & L. Camaioni (Eds.), New perspectives in early communicative development (pp. 9-22). London: Routledge.

Frith, C. D. (1992) The cognitive neuropsychology of schizophrenia. Hove, UK, and Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Gallese, V., & Goldman, A. (1998). Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mindreading. Trends in Cognitive Science, 2, 493-501.

Gallese, V., Keysers, C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2004). A unifying view of the basis of social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 396-403.

Garfield, J. L., Peterson, C. C., & Perry, T. (2001). Social cognition, language acquisition and the dev elopment of the theory of mind. Mind and Language, 16, 494-541.

Gergely, G., & Watson, J. (1996). The social biofeedback model of parental affect-mirroring. International Journal of Psycho-Analysis, 77, 1181-1212.

Gergely, G., & Watson, J. (1999). Early social-emotional development: contingency perception and the social biofeedback model. In P. Rochat (Ed.), Early social cognition: understanding others in the first months of life (pp. 101-137). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Goldman, A. (1993). The psychology of folk psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 15-28.

Gopnik, A. (1993). How we know our minds. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 1-14.

Gordon, R. M. (1986). Folk psychology as simulation. Mind and Language, 1, 158-171.

Gordon, R. M. (1996). Radical simulationism. In P. Carruthers & P. K. Smith (Eds.), Theories of theories of mind (pp. 11-21). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Grice, H. P. (1957). Meaning. The Philosophical Review, 67, 377-388.

Grice, H. P. (1982). Mean ing rev isited. In N. V. Smith (Ed.), Mutual knowledge (pp. 222-243). New York: Academic Press.

Grice, H. P. (1989). Studies in the way of words. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Guidano, V. F. (1987). Complexity of the self: a developmental app roach to psychopathology and therapy. New York: Guilford.

Guidano, V. F. (1991). The self in process. Toward a post-rationalist cognitive therapy. New York: Guilford.

Harris, P. L. (1991). The work of the imagination. In A. Whiten (Ed.), Natural theorie s of min d (pp. 283-304). Oxford: Blackwell.

Hobson, R. P. (1993). Autism and the development of mind. Hove, UK: Erlbaum.

Jacob, P., & Jeannerod, M. (2005). The motor theory of social cognition: a critique. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 21-25.

Johns on, M. (1 987). The body in the mind: the bodily basis of imagination, reason and meaning. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Leslie, A. M. (1994a). Pretending and believing: issues in the theory of ToMM. Cognition, 50, 211-238.

Leslie, A. M. (19 94b). ToMM, ToBy, and agency: core architecture and domain specificity. In L. A. Hirschfeld & S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind. Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 119-148). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Lewontin, R. (2000). Th e triple helix: gene, organism and environment. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Meltzoff, A. N., & Gopnik, A. (1993). The role of imitation in understanding persons and developing theories of mind. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds (pp. 335-366). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1983). Newborn infants imitate adult facial gestures. Child Development, 54, 702-709.

Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1989). Imitation in newborn infants: exploring the range of gestures imitated and the underlying mechanisms. Developmental Psychology, 25, 954-962.

Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1997). Explaining facial imitation: theoretical model. Early Development and Parenting, 6, 179-192.

Minsky, M. (1985). The society of mind. New York: Simon and Schuster.

Morton, J., & Johnson, M. H. (1991). CONSPECT and CONLEARN: a two process theory of infant face recognition. Psychological Review, 98, 164-181.

Murray, L., & Trevarthen, C. (1985). E motional regulation of interaction between two-month-olds and their mothers. In T. M. Field & N. A. Fox (Eds.), Social perception in infant (pp. 177-197). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

Nagel, T. (1986). The view from nowhere. Oxford: Oxford University P ress.

Papo usek, H., & Papousek, M. (1974). Mirror-image and self recognition in young infants: a new method of experimental analysis. Developmental Psychobiology, 7, 149-157.

Premack, D. (1988). 'Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind?' revisited. I n R. W. Byrne & A. Whiten (Eds.), Machiavellian intelligence: social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes, and humans (pp. 161-179). Oxford: Clarendon Press.

Premack, D. (1990). The infant's theory of self-propelled objects. Cognition, 36, 1-16.

Rao, A., & Georgeff, M. (1992). An abstract architecture for rational agents. In B. Nebel, C. Rich, & W. Swartout (Eds.), 3rd International conference on knowledge representation and reasoning (pp. 439-449). San Mateo, CA: Morgan Kaufmann.

Reddy, V., H ay, D., Murray, L., & Trevarthen, C. (1997). Communication in infancy: mutual regulation of affect and attention. In G. Bremner, A. Slater, & G. Butterworth (Eds.), Infant development: recent advances (pp. 247-273). Hove: Psychology Press.

Risjord, M. (1996). Meaning, belief, and language acquisition. Philosophical Psychology, 9, 465-475.

Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, V., & Fogassi, L. (1996). Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Cognitive Brain Research, 3, 131-141.

Rochat, P., & Morgan, R. (1995). Spatial determinants in the perception of self-produced leg movements in 3- to 5-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 31, 626-636.

Schiffer, S. R. (1972). Meaning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Searle, J. R. (1969). Speech acts: an essay in the philosophy of language.

London: Cambridge University Press.

Searle, J. R. (1979). Expression and meaning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Searle, J. R. (1980). Minds, brains, and programs. Behavioral and Bra in Sciences, 3, 417-456.

Searle, J. R. (1983). Intentionality: an essay in the philosophy of mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Searle, J. R. (1992). The rediscovery of the mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (1986). Rel evance. Communication and cognition. Oxford: Blackwell.

Stern, D. N. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant. New York: Basic Books.

Stern, D. N. (1995). Self/other differentiation in the domain of intimate socio-affective interaction: some consid erations. In P. Rochat (Ed.), The self in infancy: theory and research (pp. 419-429). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Strawson, P. F. (1964). Intention and convention in speech acts. The Philosophical Review, 73, 439-460.

Thompson, R. (1998). Empathy and its origins in early d evelopment. In S. Braten (Ed.), Intersubjective communication and emotion in early ontogeny (pp. 144-157). Paris: Cambridge University Press.

Tirassa, M. (1997). Mental states in communication. 2nd European conference on cognitive science. Manchester, UK, 103-114.

Tirassa, M. (1999a). Communicative competence and the architecture of the mind/brain. Brain and Language, 6, 419-441.

Tirassa, M. (1999b). Taking the trivial doctrine seriously: functionalism, eliminativism, and materialism. Beh avioral and Br ain Sciences, 22, 851-852.

Tirassa, M. (in press). Agencies. In L. A. Perez Miranda, & J. M. Larrazabal (Eds.), Advances in cognitive science. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

Tirassa, M., Bosco, F. M., & Colle, L. (2006). Rethinking the ontogeny of mi ndreading. Cons ciousness and Cognition, 15, 197-217.

Tirassa, M., Carassa, A., & Geminiani, G. (2000). A theoretical framework for the study of spatial cognition. In S. O'Nuallain (Ed.), Spatial cognition. Foundations and applications (pp. 19-31). Amsterdam and Philade lphia: Benjamins.

Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In C. Moore & P. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: its origins and role in development (pp. 103-130). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Tomasello, M., & Haberl, K. (2003). Understanding at tention: 12- and 18-month-olds know what is new for other persons. Developmental Psychology, 39, 906-912.

Trevarthen, C. (1977). Descriptive analyses of infant communicative behavior. In H. Schaffer (Ed.), Determinants of infant behavio r (pp. 227-270). L ondon: Academic.

Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and cooperation in early infancy: a description of primary intersubjectivity. In M. Bullowa (Ed.), Before speech. The beginning of interpersonal communication (pp. 321-347). Cambrid ge: Cambridge Unive rsity Press.

Trevarthen, C. (1993a). The function of emotions in early infant communication and development. In J. Nadel & L. Camaioni (Eds.), New perspectives in early communicative development (pp. 48-81). London: Routledge.

Trevarthen, C. (1993b). Th e self born in intersubjectivity: the psychology of an infant communicating. In U. Neisser (Ed.), The perceived self: ecological and interpersonal sources of self-knowledge (pp. 121-173). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Varela, F. (1996). A science of consciousness as if experience mattered. In S. R. Hameroff, A. W. Kaszniak, & A. C. Scott (Eds.), Toward a science of consciousness (pp. 31-44). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Varela, F. J., Thompson, E., & Rosch, E. (1991). The embod ied mind. Cognitive s cience and human experience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

Vygotsky, L. S. (1981). The genesis of higher mental functions. In J. V. Wertsch (Ed.), The concept of activity in Soviet psychology (pp. 144-188). Armonk, NY: M.E. Sharpe.

l

Metadata

Repository Staff Only: item control page